Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026//0044-3409.211.4.193

Zusammenfassung. Arbeitsgedächtnismodelle betonen meist die Bedeutung des Arbeitsgedächtnisses als Informationsspeicher für höhere kognitive Aufgaben. Wir betrachten in unseren aktuellen Arbeiten zum Arbeitsgedächtnis einen anderen Aspekt: Aufbauend auf theoretischen Positionen, die eine enge Verbindung zwischen dem Arbeitsgedächtnis und dem Aufmerksamkeitssystem postulieren (z.B. Cowan, 1999), untersuchen wir die unwillkürliche Aufmerksamkeitsablenkung im Zusammenspiel mit zentralen Steuerprozessen als Grundlage für flexibles Verhalten. Dabei zeigt sich, dass neben den bekannten Speicher- und Kontrollprozessen, die Einstellung der Distrahierbarkeit und die willkürliche Reorientierung auf aufgabenspezifische Information wichtige Aspekte des Arbeitsgedächtnis darstellen. Insgesamt ergibt sich ein differenziertes Bild der Interaktion verschiedener Prozesse (Informationsspeicherung, Aufmerksamkeitssteuerung, sensorische Verarbeitung), das wir in einem Arbeitsgedächtnismodell zusammenfassen.


Sensory memory and attentional control as a pre-requisite for working memory processes

Abstract. Typically, working memory models stress the function of maintaining information in the service of higher cognitive functions. We discuss another aspect of working memory: In accordance with models suggesting a close interaction of working memory and the attentional system (i.e. Cowan, 1999) we investigate the interaction of involuntary attention and central executive processes as a prerequisite for both information maintenance and flexible reaction on unexpected changes in the environment. The present review shows the importance of controlling the effects of pre-attentive sensory processing and the voluntary re-orienting to task-relevant information. It leads to an integrative working memory model including information storage, attention, and sensory processing.

Literatur

  • Ahveninen, J. , Jääskeläinen, I. P. , Pekkonen, E. , Hallberg, A. , Hietanen, M. , Näätänen, R. , Schröger, E. , Sillanaukee, P. (2000). Increased distractibility by task-irrelevant sound changes in abstinent alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24 , 1850– 1854 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. , Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, VIII (pp. 47-89). New York, NJ: Academic Press First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory . Oxford Psychology Series, 11. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(A) , 5– 28 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berti, S. , Schröger, E. (2001a). A comparison of auditory and visual distraction: behavioral and event-related indices. Cognitive Brain Research, 11 , 265– 273 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berti, S. , Schröger, E. (2001b). Involuntary attention switch with different levels of distractor strength. In E. Sommerfeld, R. Kompass & T. Lachmann (Eds.), Fechnerday 2001 - Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 474-478). Lengrich u.a.: Pabst Science Publishers First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Berti, S. , Schröger, E. (2003). Working memory controls in-voluntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17 , 1119– 1122 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berti, S. , Schröger, E. (2002). Distraction of working memory processing and task load . Manuskript in Vorbereitung First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cowan, N. (1998). Visual and auditory working memory capa-city. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2 , 77– 78 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (pp. 62-101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Daneman, N. , Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19 , 450– 466 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Daneman, M. , Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3 , 422– 433 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engle, R. W. , Kane, M. J. , Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and function of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (S. 102-134). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Escera, C. , Alho, K. , Schröger, E. , Winkler, I. (2001). Involuntary attention and distractability as evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiology & Neuro-Otology, 5 , 151– 166 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Escera, C. , Yago, E. , Alho, K. (2001). Electrical responses reveal the temporal dynamics of brain events during involuntary attention switching. European Journal of Neuroscience, 14 , 877– 883 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, P. , Cycowic, Y. M. , Gaeta, H. (2001). Novelty P3: an event-related brain potential ERP sign of the brain’s evaluation of novelty. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25 , 355– 373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26 , 263– 276 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goschke, T. (1996). Wille und Kognition: Zur funktionalen Architektur der intentionalen Handlungssteuerung. In J. Kuhl & K. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Themenbereich C Theorie und Forschung, Serie IV Motivation und Emotion, Band 4 Motivation, Volition und Handlung (S. 583-663). Göttingen: Hogrefe-Verlag First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jääskeläinen, I. P. , Schröger, E. , Näätänen, R. (1999). Effects of ethanol on auditory distraction: an ERP and behavioral study. Psychopharmacology, 141 , 16– 21 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McElree, B. (2001). Working memory and focal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27 , 817– 835 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. (1992). Attention and Brain Function . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. , Winkler, I. (1999). The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 125 , 826– 859 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28 , 411– 421 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roeber, U. , Berti, S. , Schröger, E. (2003). Auditory distraction with different presentation rates: An event-related potential and behavioral study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114 , 341– 349 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rösler, F. , Heil, M. (1998). Kognitive Psychophysiologie. In F. Rösler (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Themenbereich C Theorie und Forschung, Serie I Biologische Psychologie, Band 5 Ergebnisse und Anwendungen der Psychophysiologie (S. 165-224). Göttingen: Hogrefe-Verlag First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. (1997). On the detection of auditory deviants: A pre-attentive activation model. Psychophysiology, 34 , 245– 257 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Giard, M. H. , Wolff, C. (2000). Auditory distraction: event-related potential and behavioral indices. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111 , 1450– 1460 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Wolff, C. (1998a). Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. Cognitive Brain Research, 7 , 71– 87 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Wolff, C. (1998b). Attentional orienting and reorienting by human event-related brain potentials. NeuroReport, 9 , 3355– 3358 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröger, E. , Berti, S. (2000). Distracting working memory by automatic deviance-detection in audition and vision. In E. Schröger, A. Mecklinger & A. D. Friederici (Eds.), Working on Working Memory (pp. 1-25). Leipzig: Leipziger Universitätsverlag First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sussman, E. , Winkler, I. , Schröger, E. (2002). Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Manuskript in Druck First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tales, A. , Newton, P. , Troscianko, T. , Butler, S. (1999). Mismatch negativity in the visual modality. NeuroReport, 10 , 3363– 3367 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar