Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Light Pollution Modifies the Expression of Daily Rhythms and Behavior Patterns in a Nocturnal Primate

  • Thomas Le Tallec,

    Affiliation Mécanismes Adaptatifs et Evolution, UMR 7179, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Muséum National d'Histoire Naturelle, Brunoy, France

  • Martine Perret,

    Affiliation Mécanismes Adaptatifs et Evolution, UMR 7179, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Muséum National d'Histoire Naturelle, Brunoy, France

  • Marc Théry

    thery@mnhn.fr

    Affiliation Mécanismes Adaptatifs et Evolution, UMR 7179, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Muséum National d'Histoire Naturelle, Brunoy, France

Abstract

Among anthropogenic pressures, light pollution altering light/dark cycles and changing the nocturnal component of the environment constitutes a threat for biodiversity. Light pollution is widely spread across the world and continuously growing. However, despite the efforts realized to describe and understand the effects of artificial lighting on fauna, few studies have documented its consequences on biological rhythms, behavioral and physiological functions in nocturnal mammals. To determine the impacts of light pollution on nocturnal mammals an experimental study was conducted on a nocturnal primate, the grey mouse lemur Microcebus murinus. Male mouse lemurs (N = 8) were exposed 14 nights to moonlight treatment and then exposed 14 nights to light pollution treatment. For both treatments, chronobiological parameters related to locomotor activity and core temperature were recorded using telemetric transmitters. In addition, at the end of each treatment, the 14th night, nocturnal and feeding behaviors were explored using an infrared camera. Finally, throughout the study, body mass and daily caloric food intake were recorded. For the first time in a nocturnal primate, light pollution was demonstrated to modify daily rhythms of locomotor activity and core temperature especially through phase delays and increases in core temperature. Moreover, nocturnal activity and feeding behaviors patterns were modified negatively. This study suggests that light pollution induces daily desynchronization of biological rhythms and could lead to seasonal desynchronization with potential deleterious consequences for animals in terms of adaptation and anticipation of environmental changes.

Introduction

Biodiversity faces a global decline. Most indicators of biodiversity show decreasing trends whereas most indicators of pressures on biodiversity show increasing trends [1]. Urbanization, suburbanization and associated anthropogenic activities exercise a strong constraint on living organisms and ecosystem functioning. These pressures can be direct (e.g. habitat modification and fragmentation) or indirect (e.g. altered temperature; air, noise and light pollution) [2]. Among anthropogenic pressures, light pollution exerts a strong force of selection on biodiversity [3][6]. Described for the first time in the 1880s [7], when Thomas Edison marketed the first electric bulbs, the effects of light pollution on living organisms and ecosystems were nevertheless long underestimated. A century later, Smith defined light pollution when the artificial sky brightness is greater than 10 per cent of the natural night sky brightness above 45° of elevation [8]. According this definition, Cinzano et al. [9] characterized the extent of light pollution across the world showing that light pollution impacted 85.3% of the surface area in the European Union, 61.8% in the United States (excluding Alaska and Hawaii) and 18.7% of emerged lands at the global scale. In addition, the increase of light pollution was estimated around 10% per year in the European countries [9], [10]. Referring to these studies, it could be estimated that the European countries are saturated by light pollution today. More recently, the increase of light pollution was estimated near 6% per year (range: 0–20%) across the world [3]. However, altering daily and seasonal cycles of natural light, light pollution impacts living organisms and ecosystems. Since several decades, it has been shown that light pollution affects most taxa (vertebrates, invertebrates and plants [11]). In addition, 28% of vertebrates and 64.4% of invertebrates live exclusively or partially at night, i.e. many species susceptible to be disturbed [4]. Most studies of light pollution have been conducted on nocturnal insects, reptiles (mainly sea turtles) and birds. Attraction of nocturnal insects to streetlights [12], [13], alteration of reproductive behavior in sea turtles near illuminated beaches [14] and misorientation/disorientation of birds near urban sky glow are the best documented impacts of light pollution [15], [16]. In contrast, the effects of light pollution on mammals have been largely underinvestigated [17].

In mammals, as in most animals, natural light and its cycles are fundamental for spatial and temporal representation of the environment. Natural light supplies crucial visual information and also non-visual information required to synchronize the internal biological clock with the geophysical cycles on Earth. In terms of chronobiology, i.e. in terms of temporal organization of living organisms, this synchronization allows to adapt to the environment [18][20]. Under light pollution, could the light information perceived by mammals be modified and the daytime artificially lengthened leading to disturbed behaviors, biological rhythms and physiological functions as suggested by Shuboni et al. [21]? Most studies on light pollution effects in mammals have been conducted in rodents and bats. They stressed a decrease of locomotor activity and a delay of daily emergence activity [22][26], a reduction of foraging and modifications of feeding behavior [25], [27][29] and a negative impact on juvenile growth, body mass and immune function [23], [30] after exposure to light pollution. However, to our knowledge, except recent works as those conducted by Rotics et al. [26], [31], few experimental studies explore in detail the impact of light pollution on biological rhythms in mammals, especially on daily rhythms of locomotor activity and core temperature, both parameters conventionally used in chronobiology.

To better understand the consequences of urban light pollution on mammals, we conducted an experimental study on a nocturnal primate, the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). The grey mouse lemur, a Malagasy prosimian species representative of the common ancestor of primates [32], is a convenient model to study the impact of light pollution because of the strong dependence of its behaviors, biological rhythms and physiological functions on the photoperiod in the wild and in captivity. The annual rhythm of the mouse lemur is characterized by an active sexual state in long-days photoperiod (>12 hours light), an inactive sexual state and an increase in adipose mass in short-days photoperiod (<12 hours light) [33]. The daily rhythm is characterized by an important locomotor activity with euthermy at night and low locomotor activity with a phase of hypothermia during the day [34]. In this study, we used a quantitative approach based on chronobiological tools to investigate the impact of light pollution on parameters related to daily rhythms of locomotor activity and core temperature. Jointly we observed nocturnal and feeding behaviors in relation to body mass. Considering the dependence of daily rhythms on photoperiod and considering that light pollution could modify the light information perceived by animals and artificially lengthen daytime, we predicted a phase delay in daily rhythms of locomotor activity and core temperature and a negative effect on nocturnal and feeding behaviors during exposure to light pollution.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

All experiments were performed in the laboratory breeding-colony of Brunoy (UMR 7179 CNRS/MNHN, France; agreement n° E91-114-1 from the Direction Départementale de la Protection des Populations de l'Essonne) under the authorization n° Ce5/2011/067 from the Charles Darwin Ethics Committee in Animal Experiment and the Internal Review Board of the UMR 7179. All experiments were done under personal license to T. Le Tallec (authorization n° A91–621 from the Direction Départementale de la Protection des Populations de l'Essonne). Surgery was performed under veterinary supervision (DVM F. Aujard n° 17–145) and all efforts were made to minimize nociception. Human endpoints were determined as follows: significant loss of body mass (body mass <60 g); inactivity associated with a state of prostration. During the study no human endpoints were reached and no animal was sacrificed.

Animals and housing conditions

To avoid confounding effects associated with urban areas, the study was conducted on captive animals. Eight adult (37.5±2.1 months) male grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) were studied. Animals were born in the laboratory breeding-colony of the National Museum of Natural History in Brunoy, France (48°41′52″ N, 2°30′16″ E) from a stock originally caught in southern Madagascar 45 years ago. Animals were exposed to a natural photoperiodic regimen to entrain the seasonal variations of behaviors, daily rhythms and physiological functions, and were studied during the short-days season, from 8th January to 4th February 2012 (mean civil dawn: 7:56±0:02 AM Gmt+1, mean civil dusk: 6:06±0:03 PM Gmt+1 [35]; civil twilight corresponds to a sun 6° below horizon). They were housed individually in cages (50×30×30 cm), with branches and a nest box, to minimize social influences on biological rhythms. General conditions of captivity were maintained constant: ambient temperature (16–18°C), relative humidity (55%), food in excess including a homemade milky mixture (46 kJ/day) and fresh fruits (29,5 kJ/day) delivered every day during the diurnal resting phase and water available ad libitum.

Light treatments

Individual cages were positioned in an experimental room with large bay windows in front of, or away from, streetlights. Due to limited sample size (N = 8), animals were treated as their own control. In order to minimize the impact of external factors during the experiment, all efforts were realized to maintain constant the conditions of captivity. Animals were first exposed 14 nights to the moonlight treatment (MOON) (light intensity: 3.9±0.1 nmol photons.s−1.m−2 (Figure 1A); the moonlight treatment was representative of the night sky in Brunoy, with a light intensity similar to the full moon (3.5±0.1 nmol photons.s−1.m−2 [36]). Thereafter, the same animals were exposed 14 nights to the light pollution treatment (POLL) (light intensity: 24.2±0.9 nmol photons.s−1.m−2 (Figure 1B); the light pollution treatment corresponded to a streetlight located 50 m in front of the cages and positioned 8 m above the ground; spectrum type: high pressure sodium lamp, the most common artificial light used for outdoor lighting characterized by emission lines from 569 to 616 nm [37]; mean timetable switch-on: 5:32±0:03 PM Gmt+1; switch-off: 8:32±0:02 AM Gmt+1). The average light intensity and irradiance spectra were measured at night before and during the experiment using a JAZ spectrometer (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, Florida, USA) between 300–700 nm in triplicate for each individual cage. The spectrometer was placed at the center of the cage in vertical position. During daytime animals were exposed to natural daylight.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Differences in irradiance spectra of the lights used.

To test the impact of urban light pollution on Microcebus murinus, we exposed animals to (A) moonlight treatment (light intensity: 3.9±0.1 nmol photons.s−1.m−2); then to (B) light pollution treatment (light intensity: 24.2±0.9 nmol photons.s−1.m−2; light of a high pressure sodium streetlight located 50 m in front of the experimental room; peak at 592 nm).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079250.g001

Core temperature and locomotor activity recording

Throughout the study, core temperature (Tc) and locomotor activity (LA) were measured using TA10TA-F20 telemetric transmitters (Data Science Co. Ltd, Saint-Paul, Minnesota, USA) implanted under general anaesthesia (Valium: 1 mg/100 g – sub-cutaneous injection; Ketamine Imalgen: 10 mg/100 g – intra-peritoneal injection; post-operative analgesia, Meloxicam Metacam: 0.02 mg/100 g – sub-cutaneous injection) in the visceral cavity and under veterinary supervision. Calibration for each transmitter was provided by the manufacturer. Experiments were performed after at least two weeks of recovery. A receiving plate (RPC-1, Data Science Co. Ltd, Saint-Paul, Minnesota, USA) positioned in the cage allowed recording of data sent by the transmitter. Tc (in °C) was recorded every 10 min and LA (in arbitrary units) was continuously recorded and summed within this interval by antennas located in the receiving plate and detecting vertical and horizontal movements (X-Y coordinate system, Dataquest Lab Pro v 3.1, Data Science Co. Ltd, Saint-Paul, Minnesota, USA).

The following parameters were computed for each experimental treatment (MOON and POLL) and each animal, day after day, using graphic determination (Figure 2): in terms of daily rhythm of Tc, we calculated mean Tc during the active nocturnal phase (Tcactive in °C), mean Tc during the inactive diurnal phase (Tcinactive in °C), minimal Tc value (Tcmin in °C), time of occurrence of Tcmin (Hmin in min), time of onset of Tc drop (Hdecr in min), duration of the torpor bout (Torporduration in min) and frequency of the torpor bout (Torporfrequency). To determine Tcactive and Tcinactive, Tc values were averaged during the nocturnal active phase and the diurnal resting phase, respectively between the LA onset and offset and between the LA offset and onset. Hmin and Hdecr were expressed in minutes relative to civil dawn. Hdecr was determined as the first time point after which at least three successive bins of Tc decrease occurred. Hmin was determined as the time point occurring at least after 30 min of decrease and preceding at least 30 min of Tc increase. Consequently, Tcmin corresponded to the Tc value pointed at the Hmin time point [38]. Torporduration was computed as the duration with Tc below 33°C (shallow torpor [39]). Torporfrequency corresponded to the number of nights with torpor bouts for each treatment. In terms of daily rhythm of LA, LA onset and offset (LAonset, LAoffset in min) were defined respectively as the time of occurrence of the first or last three successive bins when activity was greater or lower than LA averaged on 24 hours. LAonset and LAoffset were respectively expressed in minutes relative to civil dusk and civil dawn. The duration of the LA nocturnal active phase (α in min) corresponded to the time duration between LAonset and LAoffset. To determine LA nocturnal intensity and LA diurnal intensity (LAactive and LAinactive in arbitrary units), LA values were averaged during the nocturnal active phase and the diurnal resting phase, respectively between LAonset and LAoffset and between LAoffset and LAonset [38]. For all temporal parameters (Hmin, Hdecr, LAonset and LAoffset), phase advances and phase delays were respectively expressed by positive or negative values in reference to civil twilight. Civil twilight timing was chosen as reference point because it corresponds to the most rapid changes in light intensity during the daily cycle [40].

thumbnail
Figure 2. Parameters related to daily rhythms in Microcebus murinus.

Parameters used to characterize daily rhythms of locomotor activity (LA – arbitrary units) and core temperature (Tc) in mouse lemurs during short-days photoperiod. The night period is indicated by the grey area.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079250.g002

Nocturnal and feeding behaviors recording

For each experimental treatment (MOON and POLL) and each animal, the 14th night, representative of the end of treatment, was filmed using an infrared camera (Handycam HDR-SR7, Sony, Tokyo, Japan) from 4:00 PM, before the animal emerged from its nest box, until at least 30 min after its last return inside, usually around 6:00 AM. To avoid disturbing animals, the camera was positioned during the diurnal resting phase and at one meter from the cage. After recording, the following parameters were computed: in terms of nocturnal behavior, we calculated time of occurrence of the first emergence from the nest box and time of occurrence of the last return inside (Emergeonset and Emergeoffset in min) respectively expressed relative to civil dusk and civil dawn and time spent outside the nest box (Timeoutside in min) during the nocturnal active phase. Animals were considered outside the nest box when their four legs were simultaneously visible. In terms of feeding behavior, time of occurrence of the first feeding and the last feeding bout (Feedonset and Feedoffset in min) respectively expressed relative to civil dusk and civil dawn, number of feeding bouts outside the nest box (Feedn), time spent feeding outside the nest box (Feedoutside in min) and number of fresh fruits slices brought back to the nest box (Fruitsnest) were determined. For all temporal parameters (Emergeonset, Emergeoffset, Feedonset and Feedoffset) phase advances and phase delays were respectively expressed by positive or negative values in reference to civil twilight.

Body mass and caloric intake

Before the experiment, values of body mass were controlled to ensure of their homogeneity (106.9±4.6 g). During each experimental treatment (MOON and POLL), body mass was measured at days 1, 8 and 15 and the body mass slope (Bmslope) was computed between days 1 and 15. Daily caloric food intake (CI in kJ) was calculated for each animal from the difference between provided and remaining food mass and was corrected for dehydration. Bmslope was representative of changes in body condition and CI was representative of feeding behavior.

Statistical analysis

Because we used temporal repeated measures on the same animals, Tcactive, Tcinactive, Tcmin, Hmin, Hdecr, Torporduration, LAonset, LAoffset, α, LAactive, LAinactive and CI parameters were analyzed using linear mixed effects models, built with the ‘lme’ function (package ‘nlme’) [41]. To explore the effect of light pollution on these parameters throughout the exposure and to determine if this effect could vary over time, the starting model including the effect of light pollution, the effect of time (in numbers of day since the beginning of the experiment) and the effect of the interaction between light pollution and time has been constructed. To take into account inter-individual variability, the effect of individual identity was declared as a random effect. To take into account temporal autocorrelation among model's residuals, an auto-regressive structure of order 1 (‘corAR1’ function) was included into the model. The final model, containing only significant effect(s), was obtained by deletion of the non-significant effect(s) from the starting model. Normality of final model's residuals was checked with a normal quantile-quantile plot and homoscedasticity with boxplot of parameters' residuals according to treatment and boxplot of parameters' residuals according to days [41]. Other parameters including Torporfrequency, Emergeonset, Emergeoffset, Timeoutside, Feedonset, Feedoffset, Feedn, Feedoutside, Fruitsnest and Bmslope were analyzed using, when appropriate, Student paired t-test or Wilcoxon paired test. All these parameters were checked for normality with the Shapiro-Wilk test. The probability level for statistical significance was P<0.05. All analyses were performed with R version 2.14.2 (R Development Core Team, 2001). Results are presented as means ± standard error of the means (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Means ± SEM of parameters and related statistics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079250.t001

Results

Effects of light pollution on daily rhythms: core temperature

In terms of daily rhythm of core temperature, all parameters were affected by light pollution throughout the two weeks of exposure without direct effect or interaction with time. In POLL, the mean Tc during the active nocturnal phase, the mean Tc during the inactive diurnal phase and the minimal Tc value were significantly higher than in MOON (respectively 0.3°C, 2.2°C and 4.5°C higher on average in POLL). In POLL, the time of onset of Tc drop and the time of occurrence of Tcmin were reached significantly later than in MOON (respectively 79.7 min and 36.2 min later on average in POLL). These results indicate that Hdecr and Hmin were delayed in POLL. In addition, in POLL the duration of the torpor bout and the frequency of the torpor bout were significantly lower compared to MOON (respectively 130.5 min lower and 2 times less frequent on average in POLL) (Table 1; Figure 3A,B).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Means ± SEM of daily rhythms in mouse lemurs exposed to moonlight versus light pollution treatment.

Mouse lemurs (N = 8) exposed to moonlight treatment (A) and then to light pollution treatment (B) at night (grey areas) during short-days photoperiod. Exposure to light pollution significantly delays the onset/offset of locomotor activity, decreases the nocturnal intensity of locomotor activity and increases both diurnal and nocturnal core temperatures.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079250.g003

Effects of light pollution on daily rhythms: locomotor activity

All parameters of the daily rhythm of locomotor activity, except the duration of the LA nocturnal active phase and the LA diurnal intensity, were affected by light pollution throughout the two weeks of exposure. For all parameters there was no direct effect or interaction with time. In POLL, the LA onset and the LA offset began and stopped respectively and significantly later than in MOON (respectively 56.3 min and 63.7 min later on average in POLL). However, there was no significant difference for the duration of the LA nocturnal active phase. In POLL, the LA nocturnal intensity was significantly lower compared to MOON (1.4 times lower on average in POLL). However, there was no significant difference for the LA diurnal intensity. These results indicate that light pollution had a negative effect on LA intensity at night, but not during the day (Table 1; Figure 3A,B).

Effects of light pollution on nocturnal behavior

All parameters of nocturnal behavior were affected by light pollution after two weeks of exposure. In POLL, the time of occurrence of the first emergence from the nest box and the time of occurrence of the last return inside the nest box occurred significantly later compared to MOON (respectively 75.8 min and 45.4 min later on average in POLL). In POLL, the time spent outside the nest box was significantly shorter than in MOON (2 times shorter on average in POLL). These results indicate a phase delay for Emergeonset and Emergeoffset in POLL and that light pollution had a negative effect at night on time spent outside the nest box (Table 1).

Effects of light pollution on feeding behavior and body mass

For feeding behavior, the time of occurrence of the first feeding and the last feeding bout were affected by light pollution after two weeks of exposure, but not the other parameters. In POLL, the time of occurrence of the first feeding bout and the time of occurrence of the last feeding bout occurred significantly later compared to MOON (respectively 68.4 min and 61.5 min later on average in POLL). These results indicate that Feedonset and Feedoffset were delayed in POLL. There was no significant difference in number of feeding bouts outside the nest box, time spent feeding outside the nest box, number of fresh fruits slices brought back to the nest box and in daily caloric food intake between MOON and POLL. Finally, light pollution had no effect on body mass. There was no difference in the body mass slope between MOON and POLL (Table 1).

Discussion

Effects of light pollution on daily rhythms: core temperature

For the first time it was demonstrated in a nocturnal primate that light pollution modifies the daily rhythm of core temperature. Core temperatures, both during the night and during the daily rest, were significantly higher under exposure to light pollution. In addition, the daily phase of hypothermia (torpor) was delayed and less pronounced corresponding to a long-days phenotype, i.e. a summer phenotype, suggesting an alteration of seasonal acclimatization. Indeed, in mouse lemurs, the patterns of core temperature and daily torpor are photoperiod-dependent with higher frequency and duration of torpor in short-days photoperiod compared to long-days photoperiod [34]. Furthermore, it is important to note that the increase in core temperature is not related to an exercise-associated thermogenesis or diet-induced thermogenesis since mouse lemurs exposed to light pollution were less active and did not eat more than during the moonlight treatment (see discussion below). These results suggest an alteration of thermoregulation related to exposure to light pollution. Similar results were obtained in the social vole (Microtus socialis), indicating that nocturnal light pulses may act as a stressor imposing a threat to the physiological homeostasis and, especially, negatively affect winter acclimatization of thermoregulatory mechanisms probably by mimicking summer acclimatization [42], [43]. The authors named this phenomenon ‘seasons out of time’ [43]. In mouse lemurs, thermoregulation, especially daily torpor, is an important and key mechanism to cope with adverse ambient temperature and low food availability [38], [39], [44], [45]. Consequently, increased core temperature and reduced torpor bouts under light pollution could lead to an increase in energy expenditures and, thus, could minimize successful adaptation to seasonal environmental changes [18], [19]. However, a complementary study is needed to strongly support this phenomenon of ‘seasons out of time’ in mouse lemurs. The study of seasonal photoperiodic responses as sexual activity will be very informative to illuminate this hypothesis. In this sense, we conducted a preliminary study on the impact of light pollution on reproductive parameters in both sexes (testosterone, oestradiol, oestrus cycle) in mouse lemurs. Results (unpublished data) indicated significant advances in seasonal activation of reproductive function in animals exposed to light pollution compared to control animals. These complementary results suggest that light pollution could desynchronize seasonal processes and alter seasonal acclimatization in mouse lemurs.

Effects of light pollution on daily rhythms: locomotor activity

Light pollution significantly modifies the daily rhythm of locomotor activity. Although its total duration was not modified, locomotor activity presented a delay in both its onset/offset and was significantly reduced at night under exposure to light pollution. Such changes were observed in bats [23][25] and in nocturnal rodents exposed to artificial light at night [22], [26], [30], [46][48] leading to modification of habitat use and inter/intraspecific competition [5], [6], [26]. These results could illustrate a desynchronization of the activity patterns with the geophysical cycles of the environment or a negative light masking effect as described by Rotics et al [31]. Indeed, light stimulus can override an animal's internal biological clock and consequently modify the activity patterns. In nocturnal species, light stimulus suppresses activity [49]. Furthermore, light pollution could be problematic considering activities related to moonlight. Recent studies have demonstrated in several taxa, including mammals, that moonlight and lunar cycle have an effect on activity rhythm, synchronization of reproduction, communication, navigation, habitat use, foraging and predation [17], [50][52]. Accordingly, beyond altering the natural light/dark and its perception by living organisms, light pollution could also affect the perception of moonlight and lunar cycle. Rich and Longcore evoke this phenomenon as a ‘perpetual full moon’ [5] and Cinzano et al. denounce a ‘perennial artificial moonlight’ caused by light pollution [53].

Effects of light pollution on nocturnal behavior

Changes in patterns of locomotor activity were associated with modifications in nocturnal behaviors of mouse lemurs exposed to light pollution which could suggest a behavioral inhibition. The time spent outside the nest box was significantly lower and the occurrences of the first emergence from the nest box and the last return inside were significantly delayed with light pollution. For the occurrence of the first emergence from the nest box, video recordings showed that animals exposed to light pollution spent several minutes at the entrance before emerging. This behavior has been observed in the flying squirrel (Glaucomys volans) whose emergence when exposed to artificial light was delayed by 40 minutes [47]. As in most small nocturnal mammals [17], light pollution induces a repulsive response in M. murinus. Under natural conditions, such behavior would be related to an anti-predator behavior. Indeed, light affects visual abilities of nocturnal predators and preys and the perceived risk of predation increases with illumination of the environment [54], [55]. Consequently, decreases in locomotor activities reduce the animal's ability and willingness to exploit its environment. Moreover, light pollution contributing to fragment the environment would modify dispersal in nocturnal mammals as shown in the Puma (Puma concolor) that move away from the urban artificial light to the darkest areas [56].

Effects of light pollution on feeding behavior and body mass

Light pollution partially alters feeding behavior in mouse lemurs. Animals fed significantly later, tended to spend less time feeding outside and brought more fruits in the nest box under light pollution. These changes did not result in a significant impact on the daily caloric food intake or on body mass. However, in the wild, where food availability is uncertain, it has been demonstrated in bats and nocturnal rodents that light pollution significantly reduces feeding behavior and body mass. In the Darwin's leaf-eared mouse (Phyllotis darwini), animals exposed to simulated moonlight carried 40% of their food to the refuge site against only 4% in dark conditions [57]. More, animals exposed to simulated moonlight consumed 15% less food during the experiment and lost 4.4 g in body mass in only one trial night against 1.1 g in dark conditions. In bat colonies (Myotis emarginatus, M. oxygnathus) exposed to light pollution, juvenile growth was slowed in association with reduced body mass [23]. It could be suggested that the shift in time for feeding behavior could lead animals to miss peaks of food availability and to generate interspecific competition. In natural communities, foraging times are partitioned among species that prefer different levels of lighting. But if natural lighting is altered by light pollution, this partitioning is changed and species that were not previously in competition could become competitors [5], [6]. Light pollution could consequently be detrimental to photo-sensitive species [4]. For example, the bat Pipistrellus pipistrellus, which tolerates artificial light, is able to benefit from the aggregation of nocturnal insects around streetlights but not the bat Rhinolophus hipposideros, which does not tolerate artificial light, generating competitive exclusion for food resources [27]. In an opposite way, Fonken et al. demonstrated in male mice that low levels of light at night disrupt the timing of food intake leading to increased weight gain and induce metabolic alterations as impaired glucose tolerance [58]. These results ask questions about the impact of light pollution on animal fitness, notably considering that increased body mass and reduced glucose tolerance are indicative of a prediabetic-like state [59].

Conclusions

For the first time in a nocturnal primate, urban light pollution was demonstrated to modify the expression of biological rhythms, nocturnal and feeding behaviors and to have a negative impact on thermoregulation and potentially energy balance through changes in patterns of daily torpor bouts. Evidence of desynchronization of both daily and seasonal biological rhythms could have deleterious consequences for animals, especially in terms of adaptation and anticipation of environmental changes. By altering behaviors, light pollution could affect the ability of individuals to effectively exploit their environment and its resources, and contribute to fragment the habitat particularly for photo-sensitive species. Ultimately, survival, reproduction and fitness of these species could be altered. At the ecological scale, the interspecific equilibrium could also be threatened.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Pierre-Yves Henry for his help in the first review of the paper, Fabien Pifferi and Fabienne Aujard for their assistance during the surgical operations, for their advice given throughout the experiment and in the first review of the paper. We acknowledge Pauline Vuarin for her assistance with statistics and Eric Gueton for his assistance in animal care. Finally, we acknowledge the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions and constructive criticism leading to the final version of this article.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: TLT MP MT. Performed the experiments: TLT. Analyzed the data: TLT MP MT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: TLT MP MT. Wrote the paper: TLT MP MT.

References

  1. 1. Butchart SHM, Walpole M, Collen B, Van Strien A, Scharlemann J, et al. (2010) Global biodiversity: indicators of recent declines. Science 328: 1164–1168.
  2. 2. Grimm NB, Faeth SH, Golubiewski NE, Redman CL, Wu J, et al. (2008) Global change and the ecology of cities. Science 319: 756–760.
  3. 3. Hölker F, Moss T, Griefahn B, Kloas W, Voigt CC (2010) The dark side of light: a transdisciplinary research agenda for light pollution policy. Ecology and Society 15..
  4. 4. Hölker F, Wolter C, Perkin EK, Tockner K (2010) Light pollution as a biodiversity threat. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 25: 681–682.
  5. 5. Longcore T, Rich C (2004) Ecological light pollution. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 2: 191–198.
  6. 6. Longcore T, Rich C (2006) Synthesis. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 413–430.
  7. 7. Kumlien HF (1888) Observations on bird migration in Milwaukee. The Auk 5: 325–328.
  8. 8. Smith FG (1979) Report and recommendations of IAU commission 50, reports on astronomy. IAU Transactions XVIIA: 218–222.
  9. 9. Cinzano P, Falchi F, Elvidge CD (2001) The first world atlas of the artificial night sky brightness. Monthly Notices of the Royal Astronomical Society 328: 689–707.
  10. 10. Cinzano P (2000) The growth of light pollution in north-eastern Italy from 1960 to 1995. In: Cinzano P, editor. Measuring and modelling light pollution. Memorie della Società Astronomica Italiana 71. pp. 159–165.
  11. 11. Rich C, Longcore T (2006) Introduction. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 1–13.
  12. 12. Eisenbeis G (2006) Artificial night lighting and insects: attraction of insects to streetlamps in a rural setting in germany. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 281–304.
  13. 13. Frank KD (2006) Effects of artificial night lighting on moths. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 305–344.
  14. 14. Salmon M (2006) Protecting sea turtles from artificial night lighting at Florida's oceanic beaches. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 141–168.
  15. 15. Gauthreaux SA, Belser CG (2006) Effects of artificial night lighting on migrating birds. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 67–93.
  16. 16. Montevecchi WA (2006) Influences of artificial light on marine birds. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 94–113.
  17. 17. Beier P (2006) Effects of artificial night lighting on terrestrial mammals. In: Rich C, Longcore T, editors. Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Island Press. pp. 19–42.
  18. 18. Bradshaw WE, Holzapfel CM (2007) Evolution of animal photoperiodism. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 38: 1–25.
  19. 19. Bradshaw WE, Holzapfel CM (2010) Light, time, and the physiology of biotic response to rapid climate change in animals. Annual Review of Physiology 72: 147–166.
  20. 20. Nilsson DE (2009) The evolution of eyes and visually guided behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 364: 2833–2847.
  21. 21. Shuboni D, Yan L (2010) Nightime dim light exposure alters the responses of the circadian system. Neuroscience 170: 1172–1178.
  22. 22. Kramer KM, Birney EC (2001) Effect of light intensity on activity patterns of patagonian leaf-eared mice, Phyllotis xanthopygus. Journal of Mammalogy 82: 535–544.
  23. 23. Boldogh S, Dobrosi D, Samu P (2007) The effects of the illumination of buildings on house-dwelling bats and its conservation consequences. Acta Chiropterologica 9: 527–534.
  24. 24. Stone EL, Jones G, Harris S (2009) Street lighting disturbs commuting bats. Current Biology 19: 1123–1127.
  25. 25. Stone EL, Jones G, Harris S (2012) Conserving energy at a cost to biodiversity? Impacts of LED lighting on bats. Global Change Biology 18: 2458–2465.
  26. 26. Rotics S, Dayan T, Kronfeld-Schor N (2011) Effect of artificial night lighting on temporally partitioned spiny mice. Journal of Mammology 92: 159–168.
  27. 27. Arlettaz R, Godat S, Meyer H (2000) Competition for food by expanding pipistrelle bat populations (Pipistrellus pipistrellus) might contribute to the decline of lesser horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus hipposideros). Biological Conservation 93: 55–60.
  28. 28. Bird BL, Branch LC, Miller DL (2004) Effects of coastal lighting on foraging behavior of beach mice. Conservation Biology 18: 1435–1439.
  29. 29. Polak T, Korine C, Yair S, Holderied MW (2011) Differential effects of artificial lighting on flight and foraging behaviour of two sympatric bat species in a desert. Journal of Zoology 285: 21–27.
  30. 30. Bedrosian TA, Fonken LK, Walton JC, Nelson RJ (2011) Chronic exposure to dim light at night suppresses immune responses in Siberian hamsters. Biology Letters 7: 468–471.
  31. 31. Rotics S, Dayan T, Levy O, Kronfeld-Schor N (2011) Light masking in the field: an experiment with nocturnal and diurnal spiny mice under semi-natural field conditions. Chronobiology International 28: 70–75.
  32. 32. Dutrillaux B (1979) Chromosomal evolution in primates: tentative phylogeny from Microcebus murinus (Prosimian) to man. Human Genetics 48: 251–314.
  33. 33. Perret M (2000) Reproduction saisonnière et succès reproducteur chez un prosimien malgache, Microcebus murinus. Primatologie. pp. 45–84.
  34. 34. Perret M, Aujard F (2001) Daily hypothermia and torpor in a tropical primate: synchronization by 24-h light-dark cycle. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 281: 1925–1933.
  35. 35. Rocher P (2010) Éphémérides pour 2012 à Paris. 1–62. Available: http://www.imcce.fr/~procher/phenomenes/Visibilit%E9_paris_2012.pdf.
  36. 36. Somanathan H, Maria R, Warrant EJ (2008) Nocturnal bees learn landmark colours in starlight. Current Biology 18: 996–997.
  37. 37. Elvidge CD, Keith DM, Tuttle BT, Baugh KE (2010) Spectral identification of lighting type and character. Sensors 10: 3961–3988.
  38. 38. Terrien J, Zizzari P, Epelbaum J, Perret M, Aujard F (2009) Daily rhythms of core temperature and locomotor activity indicate different adaptive strategies to cold exposure in adult and aged mouse lemurs acclimated to a summer-like photoperiod. Chronobiology International 26: 838–853.
  39. 39. Génin F, Perret M (2003) Daily hypothermia in captive grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus): effects of photoperiod and food restriction. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part B: Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 136: 71–81.
  40. 40. Daan S, Aschoff J (1975) Circadian rhythms of locomotor activity in captive birds and mammals: their variations with season and latitude. Oecologia 18: 269–316.
  41. 41. Zuur A, Ieno E, Walker N, Saveliev A, GM S (2009) Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. In: Gail M, Krickeberg K, Samet JM, Tsiatis A, Wong W, editors. Statistics for biology and health. Springer.
  42. 42. Zubidat AE, Ben-Shlomo R, Haim A (2007) Thermoregulatory and endocrine responses to light pulses in short-day acclimated social voles (Microtus socialis). Chronobiology International 24: 269–288.
  43. 43. Haim A, Shanas U, Zubidad Ael S, Scantelbury M (2005) Seasonality and seasons out of time: the thermoregulatory effects of light interference. Chronobiology International 22: 59–66.
  44. 44. Giroud S, Blanc S, Aujard F, Bertrand F, Gilbert C, et al. (2008) Chronic food shortage and seasonal modulations of daily torpor and locomotor activity in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 294: R1958–1967.
  45. 45. Giroud S, Perret M, Stein P, Goudable J, Aujard F, et al. (2010) The grey mouse lemur uses season-dependent fat or protein sparing strategies to face chronic food restriction. PloS One 5: e8823.
  46. 46. Brillhart DB, Kaufman DW (1991) Influence of illumination and surface structure on space use by prairie deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii). Journal of Mammalogy 72: 764–768.
  47. 47. DeCoursey PJ (1986) Light-sampling behavior in photoentrainment of a rodent circadian rhythm. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 159: 161–169.
  48. 48. Falkenberg JC, Clarke JA (1998) Microhabitat use of deer mice: effects of interspecific interaction risks. Journal of Mammalogy 79: 558–565.
  49. 49. Aschoff J (1960) Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 25: 11–28.
  50. 50. Evans JA, Elliott JA, Gorman MR (2007) Circadian effects of light no brighter than moonlight. Journal of Biological Rhythms 22: 356–367.
  51. 51. Kronfeld-Schor N, Dominoni D, de la Iglesia H, Levy O, Herzog ED, et al. (2013) Chronobiology by moonlight. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280: 1–11.
  52. 52. Dacke M, Byrne MJ, Baird E, Scholtz CH, Warrant EJ (2011) How dim is dim? Precision of the celestial compass in moonlight and sunlight. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 366: 697–702.
  53. 53. Cinzano P, Falchi F, Elvidge CD (2001) Moonlight without the moon. Earth, Moon and Planets 85: 517–522.
  54. 54. Lima SL, Dill L (1990) Behavorial decisions made under the risk predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68: 619–640.
  55. 55. Lima SL (1998) Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions. BioScience 48: 25–34.
  56. 56. Beier P (1995) Dispersal of juvenile cougars in fragmented habitat. Journal of Wildlife Management 59: 228–237.
  57. 57. Vasquez RA (1994) Assessment of predation risk via illumination level: facultative central place foraging in the cricetif rodent Phyllotis darwini. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 34: 375–381.
  58. 58. Fonken LK, Workman JL, Walton JC, Weil ZM, Morris JS, et al. (2010) Light at night increases body mass by shifting the time of food intake. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107: 18664–18669.
  59. 59. Kahn S, Hull R, Utzschneider K (2006) Mechanisms linking obesity to insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes. Nature 444: 840–846.