Skip to main content
Log in

The plausibility of visual information for hand ownership modulates multisensory synchrony perception

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We are frequently changing the position of our bodies and body parts within complex environments. How does the brain keep track of one’s own body? Current models of body ownership state that visual body ownership cues such as viewed object form and orientation are combined with multisensory information to correctly identify one’s own body, estimate its current location and evoke an experience of body ownership. Within this framework, it may be possible that the brain relies on a separate perceptual analysis of body ownership cues (e.g. form, orientation, multisensory synchrony). Alternatively, these cues may interact in earlier stages of perceptual processing—visually derived body form and orientation cues may, for example, directly modulate temporal synchrony perception. The aim of the present study was to distinguish between these two alternatives. We employed a virtual hand set-up and psychophysical methods. In a two-interval force-choice task, participants were asked to detect temporal delays between executed index finger movements and observed movements. We found that body-specifying cues interact in perceptual processing. Specifically, we show that plausible visual information (both form and orientation) for one’s own body led to significantly better detection performance for small multisensory asynchronies compared to implausible visual information. We suggest that this perceptual modulation when visual information plausible for one’s own body is present is a consequence of body-specific sensory predictions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Apps MA, Tsakiris M (2013) The free-energy self: a predictive coding account of self-recognition. Neurosci Biobeh Rev 41:85–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391(6669):756

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brozzoli C, Gentile G, Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH (2011) FMRI adaptation reveals a cortical mechanism for the coding of space near the hand. J Neurosci 31(24):9023–9031

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bubic A, von Cramon DY, Schubotz RI (2010) Prediction, cognition and the brain. Front Hum Neurosci 4:25

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • di Pellegrino G, Ladavas E, Farne A (1997) Seeing where your hands are. Nature 388(6644):730

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson HH (2012) The concept of body ownership and its relation to multisensory integration. In: Stein BE (ed) The new handbook of multisensory processing. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson HH, Spence C, Passingham RE (2004) That’s my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science 305(5685):875–877

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farne A, Pavani F, Meneghello F, Ladavas E (2000) Left tactile extinction following visual stimulation of a rubber hand. Brain 123(Pt 11):2350–2360

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folegatti A, de Vignemont F, Pavani F, Rossetti Y, Farne A (2009) Losing one’s hand: visual-proprioceptive conflict affects touch perception. PLoS ONE 4(9):e6920

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friston K (2010) The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci 11(2):127–138

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentile G, Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH (2011) Integration of visual and tactile signals from the hand in the human brain: an FMRI study. J Neurophysiol 105(2):910–922

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano MS (1999) Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96(18):10418–10421

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano MS, Hu XT, Gross CG (1997) Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. J Neurophysiol 77(5):2268–2292

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano MS, Gross CG, Taylor CSR, Moore T (2004) A system of multimodal areas in the primate brain. In: Spence C, Driver J (eds) Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 51–67

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Holle H, McLatchie N, Maurer S, Ward J (2011) Proprioceptive drift without illusions of ownership for rotated hands in the “rubber hand illusion” paradigm. Cognit Neurosci 2(3–4):171–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes NP, Snijders HJ, Spence C (2006) Reaching with alien limbs: visual exposure to prosthetic hands in a mirror biases proprioception without accompanying illusions of ownership. Percept Psychophys 68(4):685–701

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover AE, Harris LR (2012) Detecting delay in visual feedback of an action as a monitor of self recognition. Exp Brain Res 222(4):389–397

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover AE, Harris LR (2015) The role of the viewpoint on body ownership. Exp Brain Res 233(4):1053–1060

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyvarinen J (1981) Regional distribution of functions in parietal association area 7 of the monkey. Brain Res 206(2):287–303

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ide M (2013) The effect of anatomical plausibility of hand angle on the rubber-hand illusion. Perception 42(1):103–111

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalckert A, Ehrsson HH (2012) Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: a dissociation of ownership and agency. Front Hum Neurosci 6:40

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalckert A, Ehrsson HH (2014) The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership. Conscious Cogn 26:117–132

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keetels M, Vroomen J (2011) No effect of synesthetic congruency on temporal ventriloquism. Atten Percept Psychophys 73(1):209–218

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leube DT, Knoblich G, Erb M, Kircher TT (2003) Observing one’s hand become anarchic: an fMRI study of action identification. Conscious Cogn 12(4):597–608

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo MR, Cardozo S, Haggard P (2008) Visual enhancement of touch and the bodily self. Conscious Cogn 17(4):1181–1191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald PA, Paus T (2003) The role of parietal cortex in awareness of self-generated movements: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cereb Cortex 13(9):962–967

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Makin TR, Holmes NP, Zohary E (2007) Is that near my hand? Multisensory representation of peripersonal space in human intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 27(4):731–740

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Makin TR, Holmes NP, Ehrsson HH (2008) On the other hand: dummy hands and peripersonal space. Behav Brain Res 191(1):1–10

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parise C, Spence C (2008) Synesthetic congruency modulates the temporal ventriloquism effect. Neurosci Lett 442(3):257–261

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parise CV, Spence C (2009) ‘When birds of a feather flock together’: synesthetic correspondences modulate audiovisual integration in non-synesthetes. PLoS ONE 4(5):e5664

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J (2000) Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychol Sci 11(5):353–359

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Scandolara C, Matelli M, Gentilucci M (1981) Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behav Brain Res 2(2):147–163

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salomon R, Malach R, Lamy D (2009) Involvement of the intrinsic/default system in movement-related self recognition. PLoS ONE 4(10):e7527

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seth AK (2013) Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cogn Sci 17(11):565–573

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimada S, Qi Y, Hiraki K (2010) Detection of visual feedback delay in active and passive self-body movements. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Exp Brain Res 201(2):359–364

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C (2007) Audiovisual multisensory integration. Acoust Sci Technol 28(2):61–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M (2010) My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia 48(3):703–712

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M, Haggard P, Franck N, Mainy N, Sirigu A (2005) A specific role for efferent information in self-recognition. Cognition 96(3):215–231

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M, Carpenter L, James D, Fotopoulou A (2010) Hands only illusion: multisensory integration elicits sense of ownership for body parts but not for non-corporeal objects. Exp Brain Res 204(3):343–352

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vatakis A, Spence C (2007) Crossmodal binding: evaluating the “unity assumption” using audiovisual speech stimuli. Percept Psychophys 69:744–756

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vatakis A, Spence C (2008) Evaluating the influence of the ‘unity assumption’ on the temporal perception of realistic audiovisual stimuli. Acta Psychol 127(1):12–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vatakis A, Ghazanfar AA, Spence C (2008) Facilitation of multisensory integration by the “unity effect” reveals that speech is special. J Vis 8(9):14 1–11

  • Welch RB, Warren DH (1980) Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. Psychol Bull 88(3):638–667

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wichmann FA, Hill NJ (2001) The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Percept Psychophys 63(8):1293–1313

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zopf R, Savage G, Williams MA (2010) Crossmodal congruency measures of lateral distance effects on the rubber hand illusion. Neuropsychologia 48(3):713–725

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Macquarie University Research Development Grant (9201200328). We thank Kim Weldon and Jade Jackson for assistance running participants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Regine Zopf.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zopf, R., Friedman, J. & Williams, M.A. The plausibility of visual information for hand ownership modulates multisensory synchrony perception. Exp Brain Res 233, 2311–2321 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4300-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4300-2

Keywords

Navigation