Social Dominance and Serotonin Receptor Genes in Crayfish

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0070-2153(06)74006-6Get rights and content

Gene expression affects social behavior only through changes in the excitabilities of neural circuits that govern the release of the relevant motor programs. In turn, social behavior affects gene expression only through patterns of sensory stimulation that produce significant activation of relevant portions of the nervous system. In crayfish, social interactions between pairs of animals lead to changes in behavior that mark the formation of a dominance hierarchy. Those changes in behavior result from changes in the excitability of specific neural circuits. In the new subordinate, circuits for offensive behavior become less excitable and those for defensive behavior become more excitable. Serotonin, which is implicated in mechanisms for social dominance in many animals, modulates circuits for escape and avoidance responses in crayfish. The modulatory effects of serotonin on the escape circuits have been found to change with social dominance, becoming excitatory in dominant crayfish and inhibitory in subordinates. These changes in serotonin's effects on escape affect the synaptic response to sensory input of a single cell, the lateral giant (LG) command neuron for escape. Moreover, these changes occur over a 2‐week period and for the subordinate are reversible at any time following a reversal of the animal's status. The results have suggested that a persistent change in social status leads to a gradual change in the expression of serotonin receptors to a pattern that is more appropriate for the new status. To test that hypothesis, the expression patterns of crayfish serotonin receptors must be compared in dominant and subordinate animals. Two of potentially five serotonin receptors in crayfish have been cloned, sequenced, and pharmacologically characterized. Measurements of receptor expression in the whole CNS of dominant and subordinate crayfish have produced inconclusive results, probably because each receptor is widespread in the nervous system and is likely to experience opposite expression changes in different areas of the CNS. Both receptors have recently been found in identified neurons that mediate escape responses, and so the next step will be to measure their expression in these identified cells in dominant and subordinate animals.

Introduction

The struggle for survival and reproductive success that Darwin described is often most intense among members of the same species, all of whom are competing for the same resources within the same niche. This competition for food, shelter, and mating opportunities can become violent, but social animals have developed a variety of behavioral mechanisms to minimize violence. Among the most important of these is the social dominance hierarchy that is usually established through agonistic interactions. Once established, the hierarchy enables the participating animals to divide resources relatively peacefully, if unevenly (Wilson, 1975). This spares them from fighting and the chance of injury that for most animals in the wild leads directly to death.

The neural and neuroendocrine mechanisms that underlie social dominance are not well understood for any animal, in part because they include dynamic interactions within and between systems at all levels, from gene expression to social interactions (Fig. 1). These interactions form loops, such that cause and effect become indistinct. For example, a new social interaction (“World,” Fig. 1) will trigger sensory excitation, central activity, new motor patterns, and a behavioral response on a short timescale. The neural responses will also feed back to modify neural circuit function and structure through rapid mechanisms of plasticity and modify patterns of gene expression through slower mechanisms. New patterns of gene expression can promote structural changes through neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, apoptosis, or synapse elimination and functional changes through redistribution of receptors and ion channels, or through changes in second messenger cascades. These then alter circuit function and activity and so affect the animal's subsequent social behavior and experience.

Despite this complexity, some of the neurochemicals that appear to play significant roles in dominance behavior have been identified. Among the most prominent of these across phyla is serotonin (5‐hydroxytryptamine or 5‐HT) (Edwards 1997, Miczek 2002). In vertebrates, the correlation between dominance and serotonin is largely negative (Manuck 2006, Miczek 2006). In fish (Winberg et al., 1997) and monkeys (Fairbanks et al., 2004), levels of 5‐HT or its metabolite, 5‐HIAA, are lower in dominant animals than in subordinates. In crustaceans, an opposite trend was observed in shore crabs (Sneddon et al., 2000). In those animals, resting 5‐HT levels were found to be higher in the blood of winners than in losers, and 5‐HT levels increased more following a fight in winners than in losers. In crayfish, however, no differences in CNS levels of 5‐HT occurred between new dominant, subordinate, and control animals 24 hours after status differences were established (Panksepp et al., 2003).

The actions of serotonin are mediated through specific receptors that now number about 13 different types in vertebrates and 5 in arthropods (Manuck et al., 2006). Agonists of vertebrate 5‐HT1A, 5‐HT1B, and 5‐HT2A receptors have been shown to reduce aggression in mammals (Miczek et al., 2002) and in some instances to promote social dominance status. At the same time, they also affect other patterns of motor behavior that, given the widespread distribution of both 5‐HT projections and 5‐HT receptors within nervous systems, is not surprising (Miczek 2006, Peeke 2000).

Only two of the estimated five or more 5‐HT receptors have been identified in crayfish, and their role in formation and maintenance of dominance hierarchies has only begun to be studied. However, much is known about the social dominance behavior of crayfish, and some of the neural circuits that mediate aspects of that behavior have been described, together with their modulation by 5‐HT. This unique knowledge base provides an opportunity that is missing in other animals to study the interaction between social status, neural circuits that mediate discrete aspects of social behavior, and the serotonergic modulation of those circuits and behavior.

Here we will describe what is known of dominance hierarchies in crayfish, the neural circuits that mediate relevant behavior patterns, and the roles of serotonin in modulating those circuits and behaviors. Finally, we will describe the two 5‐HT receptors that have recently been identified in crayfish, and the roles they may play in modulating neural circuits that control behavior patterns relevant to social dominance behavior.

Section snippets

Dominance Hierarchies in Crustaceans

Dominance hierarchies are commonly observed in captured populations of decapod crustaceans, particularly in bottom‐ or shore‐dwelling or land‐dwelling animals, including many species of lobsters, crabs, crayfish, hermit crabs, and freshwater shrimp. In captivity, where resources of space, shelter, and food are limited, dominance hierarchies are readily established and maintained, at least until a dominant animal molts, when it becomes highly vulnerable. In the wild, territoriality is observed

Social Status and the Function of Neural Circuits

The changes in behavior that mark dominance hierarchy formation are produced by changes in the activity and excitability of the neural circuits that mediate them (Fig. 2). It has been possible to observe these changes in two sets of circuits, those that mediate tail flip behaviors and those that mediate responses to an unexpected touch.

Crayfish display four types of tail flip behaviors, each of which is excited by a unique set of stimuli and performs a distinct function. Offensive tail flips

Effects of Applied 5‐HT on Crustacean Behavior

Although the effects of serotonin on aggression and social dominance in crustaceans have been studied over the last 20 years, a clear picture has yet to emerge. In a seminal study, Livingstone et al. (1980) showed that acutely injected 5‐HT would quickly cause both lobsters and crayfish to adopt a posture in which the claws were held forward with depressed tips, the thorax was elevated, and the abdomen was flexed (Livingstone 1980, Tierney 2001). The elevated thorax of the 5‐HT posture was

Social Dependence of Serotonergic Modulation of Neural Circuit Function

Several behavior patterns that change with the animal's social status are mediated by neural circuits subject to serotonergic modulation. For example, LG escape is strongly inhibited in subordinates during fighting, and only weakly inhibited or facilitated in dominants (Krasne et al., 1987). 5‐HT has long been known to inhibit LG's response to inputs from primary afferents and interneurons (Glanzman and Krasne, 1997). Similarly, postural responses to an unexpected touch differ behaviorally (

5‐HT Receptors and Dominance

Across species, a set of 5‐HT receptor types coupled to various second messenger systems exert different neuromodulatory effects. In view of this, the different modulatory effects of 5‐HT on the responses of LG in socially dominant and subordinate crayfish might result from different expression patterns of two or more G‐protein–coupled 5‐HT receptors in the two classes of animals.

In vertebrates, serotonin receptors are a large family of proteins making up seven receptor classes, each with

The Links Among Social Status, 5‐HT, and 5‐HT Receptor Expression

Neuromodulators appear to be at the heart of dominance‐related behavior in many animals, including crayfish. Although the substances that promote dominance and aggression in crayfish are unknown, in vertebrates they appear to include the peptides arginine vasopressin (AVP) in mammals and arginine vasotocin (AVT) in fish. Released onto targets in the anterior hypothalamus, these substances promote aggressive, dominant‐like behavior (Hennessey 1992, Semsar 2004). In crayfish, substances like

References (75)

  • B.L. Antonsen et al.

    “Mechanisms of serotonergic modulation of the crayfish lateral giant escape circuit.”

    Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner

    (2005)
  • B.L. Antonsen et al.

    Serotonin and octopamine elicit stereotypical agonistic behaviors in the squat lobsters Munida quadrispina (Anomura, Galatheidae)

    J. Comp. Physiol. A

    (1997)
  • M. Araki et al.

    Cyclic AMP mediates serotonin‐induced synaptic enhancement of lateral giant interneuron of the crayfish

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2005)
  • N. Arfai et al.

    Social experience‐dependent changes in response of crayfish lateral giant neurons to 5‐HT may be induced by humoral factors

    Soc. Neurosci. Abstr.

    (1999)
  • B.S. Beltz

    Distribution and functional anatomy of amine‐containing neurons in decapod crustaceans

    Microsc. Res. Tech.

    (1999)
  • B.S. Beltz et al.

    Serotonin in crustacean systems: More than a half century of fundamental discoveries

  • D.A. Bergman et al.

    Field observations of intraspecific agonistic behavior of two crayfish species, Orconectes rusticus and Orconectes virilis, in different habitats

    Biol. Bull.

    (2003)
  • T. Breithaupt et al.

    Urine makes the difference: Chemical communication in fighting crayfish made visible

    J. Exp. Biol.

    (2002)
  • M. Briffa et al.

    Use of energy reserves in fighting hermit crabs

    Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.

    (2004)
  • C.A. Bruski et al.

    The importance of vision in agonistic communication of the crayfish Orconectes rusticus. Part I: An analysis of bout dynamics

    Behavior

    (1987)
  • M.C. Clark et al.

    Arthropod 5‐HT2 receptors: A neurohormonal receptor in decapod crustaceans that displays agonist independent activity resulting from an evolutionary alteration to the DRY motif

    J. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • J.F. Colas et al.

    Drosophila 5‐HT2 serotonin receptor: Coexpression with fushi‐tarazu during segmentation

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1995)
  • S.B. Doernberg et al.

    Agonistic behavior in naïve juvenile lobsters depleted of serotonin by 5,7‐dihydroxytryptamine

    J. Comp. Physiol. A

    (2001)
  • J. Drummond et al.

    Neural mechanisms of dominance hierarchies in crayfish

  • R.C. Eaton

    “Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior.”

    (1984)
  • D.H. Edwards et al.

    Metamodulation of the crayfish escape circuit

    Brain Behav. Evol.

    (2002)
  • L.A. Fairbanks et al.

    Adolescent impulsivity predicts adult dominance attainment in male vervet monkeys

    Am. J. Primatol.

    (2004)
  • R.E. Flamm et al.

    Aminergic modulation in lobster stomatogastric ganglion. I. Effects on motor pattern and activity of neurons within the pyloric circuit

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1986)
  • R.E. Flamm et al.

    Aminergic modulation in lobster stomatogastric ganglion. II. Target neurons of dopamine, octopamine, and serotonin within the pyloric circuit

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1986)
  • D.L. Glanzman et al.

    Serotonin and octopamine have opposite modulatory effects on the crayfish's lateral giant escape reaction

    J. Neurosci.

    (1983)
  • C. Goessmann et al.

    The formation and maintenance of crayfish hierarchies: Behavioral and self‐sustaining properties

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    (2001)
  • R.M. Harris‐Warrick et al.

    Neuromodulation of stomatogastric networks by identified neurons and transmitters

  • J. Herberholz et al.

    Patterns of neural circuit activation and behavior during dominance hierarchy formation in freely behaving crayfish

    J. Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • J. Herberholz et al.

    Parallel changes in agonistic and non‐agonistic behaviors during dominance hierarchy formation in crayfish

    J. Comp. Physiol. [A]

    (2003)
  • J. Herberholz et al.

    Escape behavior and escape circuit activation in juvenile crayfish during prey‐predator interactions

    J. Exp. Biol.

    (2004)
  • R. Huber et al.

    Serotonin alters decisions to withdraw in fighting crayfish, Astacus astacus: The motivational concept revisited

    J. Comp. Physiol. [A]

    (1998)
  • R. Huber et al.

    A quantitative analysis of agonistic behavior in juvenile American lobsters (Homarus americanus L.)

    Brain Behav. Evol.

    (1995)
  • Cited by (18)

    • Serotonergic control of excitability: from neuron to networks

      2020, Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      The depression of synaptic inputs by 5-HT involves other mechanisms that are independent of PKA, although PKA can be involved in depressed responses (Lee et al., 2008). Even though the 5-HT receptors in crayfish have been less characterized, it has been shown that the various responses of 5-HT involved several distinct 5-HT receptors (Edwards & Spitzer, 2006; Yeh et al., 1996, 1997). The neuromodulatory influence of 5-HT modifies the response of the network with the experience.

    • A fighter's comeback: Dopamine is necessary for recovery of aggression after social defeat in crickets

      2014, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Most behavioral theories agree that information gathered from agonistic signals exchanged during fighting is somehow assessed to time the decision to flee, and our work on crickets shows that agonistic signals act to suppress aggression of the opponent (Rillich et al., 2007; for review see Elwood and Arnott, 2012). Neuromodulators are likely to play key roles, for example serotonin and nitric oxide, which can suppress aggression in both mammals (Nelson and Trainor, 2007) and insects (Iwasaki et al., 2007; see also Edwards and Spitzer, 2006 on serotonin and social dominance in crustaceans). In this paper we specifically address the possibility that OA and/or dopamine (DA), are involved in the recovery of aggression after social defeat.

    • Evolutionary shift from fighting to foraging in blind cavefish through changes in the serotonin network

      2013, Current Biology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Even in CF, when the number of 5HT raphe neurons is experimentally reduced (cyclopamine), it seems possible to induce a small component of dominance-related aggressiveness. Across phyla, serotonin plays a significant role in dominance behavior [43–48]. Whether dominance is a cause or a consequence of the change in raphe 5HT neurotransmission and the mechanisms leading to these biochemical and behavioral changes is unclear.

    • Forgetting curve of cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus, derived by using serotonin hypothesis

      2012, Robotics and Autonomous Systems
      Citation Excerpt :

      Eq. (10) also predicts that the level of autoreceptor expression also influences the time constant of the forgetting curve. There are many past studies; the growth environment such as population density, dominance hierarchy, etc. change the level of autoreceptor expression [27,21]; the level of autoreceptor expression is affected by the social status. It suggests that animals change the time constant of forgetting by regulating receptor expression in response to social status adaptively.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text