Elsevier

Behavioural Processes

Volume 56, Issue 3, 3 December 2001, Pages 175-194
Behavioural Processes

Development of a raceway method to assess aversion of domestic fowl to concurrent stressors

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(01)00193-0Get rights and content

Abstract

The requirement for assessing the effects of stressor combinations in improving the welfare of animals has not been widely recognised. Knowledge of the effects of concurrent stressors is needed to improve environments such as transport, where animals are presented with many simultaneous challenges. However, no method for measuring the effects of different stressors with a common unit is currently available. A locomotor passive avoidance method was developed as a common currency measure of the aversion of domestic fowl to concurrent stressors, using vibrational and thermal stressors as an exemplar. Juvenile fowl, fasted overnight, were trained to run a raceway into a goal-box for small food rewards (FR1). When running consistently, the reinforcement schedule was superimposed with a FR5 treatment schedule (60 min confinement in the goal-box with either a control of no other stressors [N] or concurrent vibration and thermal stressors [VT]). Subsequent latency to return to the goal-box was recorded as a measure of aversion. The factors affecting bird response were addressed in a series of experiments to optimise the method and clarify interpretation of results. Pre-feeding (20% ration 2 h prior to testing) did not affect response, but increasing the number of treatment presentations facilitated learning and increased method sensitivity. Treatment responses were consistent across experiments; overall VT was avoided (P<0.001), but N was not. However, there was large individual variation in response to VT. A final experiment indicated that, given a visual discriminatory cue, birds were capable of learning the required association between entering the goal-box and receiving the treatment, suggesting that the delay responses were due to aversion rather than the immediate impact of treatment on ability to respond. Further work is required to test the singular stressors, but the method retains common currency potential for assessing aversion to multiple stressors.

Introduction

It is important to assess how domestic animals respond to management, transportation and handling procedures that are imposed as part of normal farming practice. Many of these procedures may be perceived as aversive, and methods for quantifying animal aversion have been developed and reviewed (Rutter, 1989, Rushen, 1996). However, environmental stressors are rarely experienced individually and to understand their true impact on animals, responses to various combinations of stressors require characterisation. To achieve this, new methods are needed to assess the effects of very different types of stressors using the same unit of currency. The combination of vibrational and thermal stressors was chosen as an exemplar to assess aversion to concurrent stressors, but is also relevant to the welfare of transported birds (Weeks and Nicol, 2000). Vibration is immediately perceived, novel and likely to be fear-eliciting, whereas there is a time-lag before a hot temperature induces heat stress, it is unlikely to induce fear and may have been experienced previously. Thus responses common to both stressors are few (Abeyesinghe et al., 2001).

A locomotor passive avoidance method was devised to circumvent these problems as a common currency of aversion. The premise of the raceway method is to titrate motivation to obtain food against motivation to avoid an aversive stimulus. The method is similar to both Miller's (1944) work on conflict behaviour, and Petherick et al.'s (1992) investigation of laying hens' motivation in response to food and a negative reinforcer. Miller trained rats to run an alleyway into a goal-box for food and, after subjecting them to a brief electric shock while eating, measured the distance from the goal-box at which they stopped on a subsequent run. This was assumed to represent the point of equal motivation to approach and avoid. However, there was no indication of the time period over which animal responses were measured or accounting of factors other than attraction and avoidance which might be influencing responses. Petherick et al. (1992) ran hens once per day and always rewarded them with food, even when they also received an aversive stimulus. It is likely that the single daily trials and the consistency of the reward interfered with the hens' ability to learn the association between the aversive stimulus and the goal-box. The risk of the aversive treatment may have been perceived as worthwhile to obtain the guaranteed food, reducing sensitivity of the measure.

In the current study, once birds were running a raceway consistently for food, a treatment schedule was imposed, which took precedence over the food reward. Subsequent latency to return to the goal-box was recorded as a measure of aversion. Birds were therefore required to associate a place (the goal-box) with both stimuli and respond to memory of the events occurring there. Assuming that both vibration and thermal stress were equally associated with the goal-box, this approach allowed equal comparison of the stressors on an equal basis and, with confinement of sufficient duration to allow the thermal stressor to take effect, over different time periods (Abeyesinghe et al., 2001). The major factors affecting bird responses in the raceway were investigated in a series of experiments. In experiment 1, the effect of hunger on technique sensitivity was examined. In experiment 2, the number of treatment exposures was increased to facilitate learning of the association between receipt of the treatment and the goal-box. In experiment 3, the ability of birds to learn the required association (goal-box: treatment) was investigated.

Section snippets

General apparatus

Each of three raceways consisted of a straight, unmarked wooden corridor measuring 0.5 m in width and height, respectively and 3.9 m in length (Fig. 1). The corridor commenced in a wooden start-box (0.5×0.5×0.4 m3), with a manually operated wooden release-door and terminated, via a transparent hinged door, in a wooden goal-box (0.5×0.5×0.5 m3) with a clear Perspex ceiling. The goal-box was separated from the corridor so it could be moved independently for the vibration treatment. Goal-box doors

Experiment 1: the effect of pre-feeding on response in the raceway

To maximise sensitivity of the method, the standard level of hunger against which the treatment responses were titrated required optimisation. If not hungry, birds would not respond for food, but if too hungry, they would pay very high costs to get it and would be likely to continue responding similarly over a range of treatment conditions. The objects of this experiment were:

  • i

    To investigate the aversion of broiler chickens to concurrently experienced raised temperature and vertical vibration at

Experiment 2: The effect of repeated treatment exposure

This study investigated the effects of repeated treatment exposure on technique sensitivity and addressed the potential problem of birds not learning the treatment: goal-box association after a single exposure. The apparatus used for experiment 1 was modified by placing rubber lining in the base of each food trough to dampen the sounds of feeding and hanging screens of pale calico (3×1 m2) either side of each raceway to reduce visual distraction.

Experiment 3: associative learning

To determine whether the delay in return to the goal-box was a non-associative response or due to a specific learned association, it was first important to determine whether the broiler chickens were capable of forming the required goal-box: treatment association. Experiment 3 was therefore undertaken on the following premise: if fowl form a specific association between the goal-box where treatment occurs and receipt of that treatment, they will not delay if, immediately after treatment, they

Response to concurrent vibrational and thermal stressors

An initial delay after the first No Stressor confinement was demonstrated by birds in all three experiments. Since this response was not maintained after subsequent exposures in experiments 2 and 3, it was most likely due to the novelty of a change in routine. Response to the concurrent Vibration/Thermal treatment was comparatively both substantial and varied. The mean response-times indicated appreciable delays after every exposure in all experiments, indicating that subjects perceived the

Acknowledgements

This study was undertaken as part of a PhD supported by Silsoe Research Institute and the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council. We are grateful to Rodger White for his statistical advice, James Francis for aid with animal husbandry, Len Burgess for his technical aid and the design, electronic and workshop groups of Silsoe Research Institute for construction and maintenance of the raceway apparata. We would also like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments on

References (23)

  • R.B. Jones

    Fear and adaptability in poultry: insights, implications and imperatives

    World Poult. Sci. J.

    (1996)
  • Cited by (13)

    • Examining affective structure in chickens: valence, intensity, persistence and generalization measured using a Conditioned Place Preference Test

      2018, Applied Animal Behaviour Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, it may be possible to modify conditioned suppression tests (Estes and Skinner, 1941) to measure facets of affect beyond valence and scale, although interpretation of such tasks is also complex, with suppressed operant responding occurring in the presence of both rewarding and aversive conditioned stimuli (Karpicke et al., 1977). And raceway or runway tests, in which animals’ learned running behaviour towards a reward is slowed by the addition of concurrent punishers, could also be developed further in these regards (e.g. see Abeyesinghe et al., 2001; Pajor et al., 2000; Rutter and Duncan, 1992). The Conditioned Place Preference test is a useful measure of affective responses to potentially punishing stimuli and has value both as a method for studying the, often complex, nature of affective structures in animals, and for the assessment of stimuli potentially relevant to animal welfare.

    • Positive affective state induced by opioid analgesia in laying hens with bone fractures

      2013, Applied Animal Behaviour Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Animal welfare researchers have for many years used preference and willingness-to-work tests to establish what features of the environment animals find rewarding (Mason et al., 1998; Abeyesinghe et al., 2001; Kirkden and Pajor, 2006; Nicol et al., 2009, 2011).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text