Skip to main content
Log in

Disposition to recognize goals in infant chimpanzees

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Do nonhuman primates attribute goals to others? Traditional studies with chimpanzees provide equivocal evidence for “mind reading” in nonhuman primates. Here we adopt looking time, a methodology commonly used with human infants to test infant chimpanzees. In this experiment, four infant chimpanzees saw computer-generated stimuli that mimicked a goal-directed behavior. The baby chimps performed as well as human infants, namely, they were sensitive to the trajectories of the objects, thus suggesting that chimpanzees may be endowed with a disposition to understand goal-directed behaviors. The theoretical implications of these results are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Parametric statistical analyses (ANOVAs and t-tests) were used with these data because there was no reason to believe the assumptions underlying such analyses were violated. Nonparametric analyses also require the homogeneity of variance assumption. Most of the hypotheses tested here were within-subjects hypothesis tests with only one degree of freedom in the numerator. These tests do not require the homogeneity of variance assumption.

References

  • Abell F, Happé F, Frith U (2000) Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn Dev 15:1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

  • Cacchione T, Krist H (2003) Understanding object relations: what chimpanzees know about support. J Comp Psychol (in press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Call J (2001) Chimpanzee social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 5:388–393

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Call J, Tomasello M (1998) Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans, chimpanzees, and human children. J Comp Psychol 112:192–206

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey S (1995) Continuity and discontinuity in development. In: Osherson D (ed) An introduction to cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

  • Castelli F, Happé F, Frith U, Frith C (2000) Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage 12:314–325

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G, Gergely G (1998) The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: a developmental hypothesis. Dev Sci 1:255–259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G, Gergely G, Biro S, Koos O, Brockbanck M (1999) Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition 72:237–267

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1987) Psychosemantics. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

  • Gergely G, Csibra G (1997) Teleological reasoning in infancy: the infant’s naive theory of rational action. A reply to Premack and Premack. Cognition 63:227–233

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Nadasdy Z, Csibra G, Biro S (1995) Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56:165–193

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser M, Carey S (1998) Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: evolutionary and developmental insights. In: Cummins D, Allen C (eds) The evolution of mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Hauser M, MacNeilage P, Ware M (1996) Numerical representations in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci 93:1514–1517

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser M, Newport E, Aslin R (2001) Segmentation of the speech stream in a nonhuman primate: statistical learning in cotton top tamarins. Cognition 78:53–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider F, Simmel M (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am J Psychol 57:243–249

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson S, Booth A, O’Hearn K (2001) Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent. Cogn Dev 16:637–656

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jolly A (1966) Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science 29:501–506

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1917) Intelligenzprüfungen an Anthropoiden. Abh Preuss Akad Wiss 1:1–213

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1921) Zur Psychologie der Schimpansen. Abh Preuss Akad Wiss 2:686–691

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1922) Zur Psychologie der Schimpansen. Psychol Forsch 1:2–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1925) The mentality of apes. Penguin, London

  • Kohts N (1924) Untersuchungen über die Erkenntnisfähigkeiten des Schimpansen. Moskow (summarized by RM Yerkes and A Petrunkevitch)., J Comp Psychol 1925:98–108)

  • Leslie A (1994) ToMM, ToBy and agency: core architecture and domain specificity. In: Hirschfeld L, Gelman S (eds) Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge University Press, New York

  • Leslie A (1995) A theory of agency. In: Sperber D, Premack D, Premack A (eds) Causal cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Leslie A, Keeble S (1987) Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition 25:265–288

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff A (1995) Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactments of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev Psychol 31:838–850

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munakata Y, Santos L, Spelke E, Hauser M, O’Reilly R (2000) Visual representation in the wild: how rhesus monkeys parse objects. J Cogn Neurosci 13:44–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols S, Stich S (2003) Mindreading. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Povinelli D, Perilloux H, Reaux J, Bierschwale D (1998) Young and juvenile chimpanzees’ reactions to intentional versus accidental and inadvertent actions. Behav Process 42:205–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack D, Premack A (1997) Motor competence as integral to attribution of goal. Cognition 63:235–242

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:516–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramus F, Hauser M, Miller C, Morris D, Mehler J (2000) Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science 288:349–351

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith P (1996) Language and the evolution of mindreading. In: Carruthers P, Smith P (eds) Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, Mass.

  • Sodian B, Thoermer C (2001) Preverbal infants’ understanding of referential gestures. First Lang 21:245–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber E (1915) Aus der Anthropoidenstation auf Teneriffa: Ziele und Aufgaben der Station sowie erste Beobachtungen an der auf ihr gehaltenen Schimpansen. Abhandlungen der königlich preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften 2:1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinkelpaugh OL (1928) An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Comp Psychol 8:197–236

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinkelpaugh OL (1932) Multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzees and monkeys. J Comp Psychol 13:207–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Uller C (1996) Origins of numerical concepts: a comparative study of human infants and nonhuman primates. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uller C (2001) Retraction notice for the brief article Goal attribution in chimpanzees by Claudia Uller and Shaun Nichols. Cognition 80:iii

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uller C, Nichols S (2000) Goal attribution in chimpanzees. Cognition 76:B27–B34

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uller C, Carey S, Xu F, Hauser M (1997) Is language needed for constructing sortal concepts? A study with nonhuman primates. In: Hughes E (ed) BUCLD Proc 21:665–677

  • Uller C, Hauser M, Carey S (2001) Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins. J Comp Psychol 115:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman H (1990) The child’s theory of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

  • Woodward A (1998) Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 69:1–34

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward A, Sommerville J (2000) Twelve-month old infants interpret action in context. Psychol Sci 2:73–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes RM (1927) The mind of a gorilla. Genet Psychol Monogr 1927:1–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes RM, Yerkes DN (1928) Concerning memory in the chimpanzee. J Comp Psychol 1928:237–271

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank Jeff Rowell and Babette Fontenot for the opportunity to run this experiment in the nursery at New Iberia Research Center (NIRC), the staff at NIRC and at the Cognitive Development Laboratory for technical and experimental support. I thank Robert McFatter for assistance with the statistical treatment of the data, and Akos Feher for stimuli development. I also thank four anonymous reviewers, and the Editor of Animal Cognition, for their insightful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. Finally, I express my deepest gratitude to all those who supported this replication. The experiment described here complies with the current laws for animal care and use of the United States and was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Louisiana at Lafayette (IACUC no. 2001–8717–034).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claudia Uller.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Uller, C. Disposition to recognize goals in infant chimpanzees. Anim Cogn 7, 154–161 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-003-0204-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-003-0204-9

Keywords

Navigation