Elsevier

Animal Behaviour

Volume 79, Issue 5, May 2010, Pages 981-991
Animal Behaviour

Articles
Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.021Get rights and content

Consensus decisions about the nature and timing of group activities allow animals to maintain group cohesiveness, but also entail costs because individuals often differ with respect to their optimal activity budgets. Two mechanisms whereby animals reach a consensus include ‘consistent leadership’, in which a single dominant individual makes the decision, and ‘variable leadership’ in which several group members contribute to the decision outcome. Sharing of consensus decisions is expected to reduce consensus costs to most group members. Both patterns are thought to emerge from the complexity of social relationships of group members. We investigated the distribution of leadership during group departures in two packs of free-ranging dogs, Canis lupus familiaris, and tested how its distribution between individuals was affected by dominance rank-related affiliative and agonistic relationships. Although leadership was not entirely concentrated on a single group member, both packs had a limited number of habitual leaders. In the largest pack, the pattern of leadership changed from ‘variable’ to nearly ‘consistent’ after its size had shrunk. Habitual leaders were usually old and high-ranking individuals. However, high-ranking dogs that received affiliative submissions in greeting ceremonies were more likely to lead than dominant dogs receiving submissions only in agonistic contexts. During resting times, habitual followers associated more closely with habitual leaders than with other followers. These results suggest that in social species collective movements may arise from the effort of subordinates to maintain close proximity with specific valuable social partners.

Section snippets

Study Area

The research was carried out in a suburban environment situated in the southwestern outskirts of Rome, Italy. The study area was about 300 ha and was delimited to the north, west and south by roads with heavy traffic and to the east by cultivated areas. The southwest sector was urbanized although not densely populated. The northeast sector was mainly occupied by a nature reserve called ‘Tenuta dei Massimi’. The habitat in the reserve consisted mainly of open grasslands with some interspersed

Distribution of Leadership within Packs

In the Corridoio pack (N = 133 collective movements) leadership was clearly not restricted to a single dog. As shown in Fig. 1a, every adult and subadult individual could be successful in recruiting at least two followers, whereas juveniles never behaved as leaders. However, individual differences in the frequency of leadership were significantly greater than expected by chance even when we restricted the analysis to adults and subadults (χ152 = 84.94, P < 0.0001). Altogether there were 275 attempts

Discussion

In this study we found that leadership during group departures in free-ranging dogs was not entirely concentrated on a single individual. Every adult and subadult individual within a given pack could successfully initiate a collective movement involving a minimum of three animals, whereas dogs younger than 1 year rarely succeeded in doing so. However, in all packs studied some individuals behaved as habitual leaders and others as habitual followers. In the two packs for which testing was

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Annamaria Andreozzi and Mirella De Paolis for helping with the dog census. Special thanks are due to Ilya Fischhoff and Amandine Ramseyer for providing stimulating discussions and relevant literature. Relevant papers were also provided by Larissa Conradt, Marion L. East and Kay E. Holekamp. We also thank Alessandro Giuliani, Mario Di Traglia and Gabriele Schino for statistical advices, and Luis Nieder for providing support and facilities. Finally, thanks are due to the

References (50)

  • S. Stueckle et al.

    To follow or not to follow: decision making and leadership during the morning departure in chacma baboons

    Animal Behaviour

    (2008)
  • M.S. de Villiers et al.

    Social dynamics and the cortisol response to immobilization stress of the African wild dog, Lycaon pictus

    Hormones and Behavior

    (1997)
  • J. Altmann

    Observational study of behavior: sampling methods

    Behaviour

    (1974)
  • M. Bekoff

    Scent-marking by free-ranging domestic dogs

    Biology of Behavior

    (1979)
  • L. Boitani et al.

    Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves

    Ethology, Ecology and Evolution

    (1995)
  • L. Boitani et al.

    Population biology and ecology of feral dogs in central Italy

  • R.W. Byrne

    How monkeys find their way: leadership, coordination, and cognitive maps of African baboons

  • R.W. Byrne et al.

    Social relationships of mountain baboons: leadership and affiliation in a non-female-bonded monkey

    American Journal of Primatology

    (1990)
  • Cafazzo, S. 2007. Dinamiche sociali in un gruppo di cani domestici (Canis lupus familiaris) liberi in ambiente...
  • Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R. & Natoli, E. In press. Dominance in relation to age, sex and competitive...
  • T.H. Clutton-Brock et al.

    Red Deer, Behavior and Ecology of Two Sexes

    (1982)
  • L. Conradt et al.

    Activity synchrony and social cohesion: a fission–fusion model

    Proceedings of the Royal Society B

    (2000)
  • L. Conradt et al.

    Group decision-making in animals

    Nature

    (2003)
  • L. Conradt et al.

    Democracy in animals: the evolution of shared group decisions

    Proceedings of the Royal Society B

    (2007)
  • I.D. Couzin et al.

    Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move

    Nature

    (2005)
  • Cited by (80)

    • Artificially elevated oxytocin concentrations in pet dogs are associated with higher proximity-maintenance and gazing towards the owners

      2021, Physiology and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, oxytocin concentrations were not associated with other behavioural synchrony and shared attention related measures. Behavioural synchrony has been hypothesized to be bidirectionally linked to affiliation and social cohesion in dogs ([14];[92]) and, since oxytocin enhances social and affiliative behaviours of dogs towards their owners [70], we predicted it would increase the behavioural synchrony of dogs with their owners. We found no association between post-administration oxytocin levels and the more stringent measure of behavioural synchrony (i.e. the time spent doing the same activity but also in proximity of the owner), nor with the dogs’ activity synchrony alone, or speed with which they changed activity following their owner's change of state (temporal synchrony component).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    S. Cafazzo is at the Dipartimento di Protezione e Valorizzazione Agroalimentare, Università di Bologna, via Fratelli Rosselli 107, 43100, Reggio Emilia, Italy.

    2

    E. Natoli is at Azienda USL Roma D, Area Dipartimentale Sanità Pubblica Veterinaria, Ospedale Veterinario, via della Magliana 856, 00148 Roma, Italy.

    View full text