Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Fitness Benefits of Mate Choice for Compatibility in a Socially Monogamous Species

  • Malika Ihle ,

    mihle@orn.mpg.de

    Affiliation Department of Behavioral Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, Germany

  • Bart Kempenaers,

    Affiliation Department of Behavioral Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, Germany

  • Wolfgang Forstmeier

    Affiliation Department of Behavioral Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, Germany

Abstract

Research on mate choice has primarily focused on preferences for quality indicators, assuming that all individuals show consensus about who is the most attractive. However, in some species, mating preferences seem largely individual-specific, suggesting that they might target genetic or behavioral compatibility. Few studies have quantified the fitness consequences of allowing versus preventing such idiosyncratic mate choice. Here, we report on an experiment that controls for variation in overall partner quality and show that zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) pairs that resulted from free mate choice achieved a 37% higher reproductive success than pairs that were forced to mate. Cross-fostering of freshly laid eggs showed that embryo mortality (before hatching) primarily depended on the identity of the genetic parents, whereas offspring mortality during the rearing period depended on foster-parent identity. Therefore, preventing mate choice should lead to an increase in embryo mortality if mate choice targets genetic compatibility (for embryo viability), and to an increase in offspring mortality if mate choice targets behavioral compatibility (for better rearing). We found that pairs from both treatments showed equal rates of embryo mortality, but chosen pairs were better at raising offspring. These results thus support the behavioral, but not the genetic, compatibility hypothesis. Further exploratory analyses reveal several differences in behavior and fitness components between “free-choice” and “forced” pairs.

Author Summary

The last half century has seen a tremendous interest in the study of mate choice and the evolution of traits that make individuals attractive to others. In some species, however, individuals can differ substantially in who they find attractive, and this variation has typically been interpreted as “mate choice for compatibility.” Here, we quantify the benefits of such mate choice in a socially monogamous passerine bird, the zebra finch. We found that pairs that resulted from free mate choice achieved a 37% higher reproductive success than pairs that were forced to mate with a randomly assigned individual. Forced pairs suffered from increased failure to fertilize eggs and from increased mortality of hatched offspring. In females, we observed a reduced readiness to copulate with the assigned partner, while males that were force‐paired showed reduced parental care and increased activity in courting extra‐pair females. These findings support the hypothesis that zebra finches choose mates on the basis of behavioral compatibility. In contrast, it appears that zebra finches have not evolved a mechanism that would allow them to select a partner with whom they could minimize the rate of embryo mortality. This argues against mate choice for genetic compatibility.

Introduction

The evolution of mate choice has been the focus of much research, and many studies have attempted, with a variety of experimental approaches, to measure the fitness benefits gained by choosy individuals (e.g., [1,27]). Those benefits can be either direct, if offspring quality or quantity is increased due to the partner’s behavior (including reproductive investment), or indirect, if offspring quality is improved by the genetic contribution of the partner. To date, the central debate has been about (i) the relative importance of direct versus indirect fitness benefits arising from the overall quality of the chosen partner (i.e., good parent versus good genes; Fig 1, vertical black arrow) [8,9], or about (ii) the relative importance of the two types of indirect benefits (i.e., good genes versus compatible genes; Fig 1, horizontal black arrow) [1013].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schematic overview of four types of potential fitness benefits of mate choice.

This study aims at separating direct from indirect benefits of mate choice for compatibility (red arrow), while experimentally controlling for effects of overall quality (red parentheses).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.g001

Several studies on mate choice have shown that, in some species, mating preferences can be largely specific to the individual [1419]. Such mate preferences may function to maximize offspring viability by bringing together compatible combinations of genes (top right in Fig 1). However, the alternative hypothesis that mate choice could lead to direct benefits arising from the phenotypic (e.g., behavioral) compatibility of the two partners (bottom right in Fig 1) has received only little attention [2026], despite suggestive evidence that the combination of both parents’ behaviors or other phenotypes can affect breeding success. Compatible partners could, for instance, be better at coordinating tasks, at sharing them or at complementing each other’s performance on various tasks [2325,2731], or they might simply be more effective at stimulating one another’s reproductive investment [3234]. Mate choice for such behavioral compatibility might be especially important in species with intense bi-parental brood care and with long-lasting, monogamous pair bonds, like humans or many bird species.

Previous experiments that aimed to quantify the fitness benefits of mate choice arising from partner compatibility typically compared two categories of individuals: those paired up with their preferred partner versus those that were given a non-preferred partner [3543], or a random partner from the population [44,45]. The problem with this approach is that the effects of individual quality and pair compatibility are confounded, because only the force-paired group includes individuals that might never have been chosen (i.e., low-quality individuals). Some studies addressed this issue by presenting evidence that the rejection of a particular mate depended on the choosing individual’s identity [46], or that non-preferred and preferred individuals did not differ in morphological traits [43]. Other studies have compared the reproductive success of a choosing individual (paired with its preferred partner) with the reproductive success of a naïve individual paired with that same (or another) preferred individual [2,4750]. In the latter design, chosen and assigned partners are on average of equal quality. However, choosing individuals were often discarded if they did not meet a certain criterion regarding their strength of preference ([2,48,49], but see [47,50]). If choosiness is associated with an individual’s quality [17,51], the selected subset of choosing individuals might differ in quality from the random pool of naïve individuals to which they are compared. However, in two experimental studies on invertebrates, none of the above issues apply; these studies found no fitness benefit of mate choice for compatibility [47,50].

Here, we employ an experimental design somewhat similar to [50], to eliminate the effect of mate quality: we compare the fitness of individuals that bred with their preferred partner with those that obtained, after having expressed their preference, the preferred partner of another individual. The main aim of this study is thus to quantify the benefits of mate choice that arise from partner compatibility, while circumventing confounding effects of variation in partner quality. The second aim of our study is to tease apart indirect compatibility advantages (compatibility of parental genes expressed in the offspring) from direct ones (parental phenotypic compatibility), using a model species in which these benefits of mate choice can be disentangled.

The zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) is a socially monogamous species with biparental care, in which partners mate for life [52]. In this species, female mate preferences are predominantly individual-specific (i.e., females show little consensus regarding which male is the most attractive) [15,5357], suggesting that they may target genetic or behavioral compatibility. In captive and wild populations, high rates of embryo and offspring mortality are found, even in the absence of inbreeding [25,52,5860]. Cross-fostering of freshly laid eggs (see [61,62] and S1 Text) showed that most of the variance in embryo mortality (before hatching) is explained by the identity of the genetic parents rather than the foster‐parents (based on n = 1,529 fertilized eggs, S1 Table), whereas most of the variance in offspring mortality (after hatching) is explained by foster-parent rather than genetic parent identity (n = 1106 offspring, S1 Table). Based on these results, we assume that, in zebra finches, embryo mortality primarily reflects genetic incompatibility (as in other species [63,64]), while offspring mortality primarily results from the behavior of the caring parents (here broadly referred to as “behavioral incompatibility”). Experimentally preventing mate choice should thus lead to an increase in embryo mortality if mate choice is targeting genetic compatibility, and to an increase in offspring mortality if mate choice is targeting behavioral compatibility. Alternatively, if individual-specific mate preferences only reflect indecision by the animal or measurement error [15], preventing mate choice would have no fitness consequences.

We studied 160 bachelor birds from a recently wild-derived population of zebra finches. Each individual could freely choose a partner from a group of 20 individuals of the opposite sex during a long, nonbreeding season. This setup reflects the natural situation in the sense that zebra finches are opportunistic breeders and do not reproduce if the environment is not suitable, but they still form life-long pair bonds irrespective of breeding opportunities. Furthermore, the species is gregarious, such that individuals have many potential partners to choose from. Pairs were identified by the occurrence of allopreening because we found that this best reflects mutual preferences rather than being the outcome of intra-sexual competition (see S2 Text). We hereafter focus on female preferences; however, because observed allopreening preferences were mainly reciprocal between females and males (see also S2 Text), any observed effects of the experimental treatment described below could be due to females, males, or both sexes not being able to breed with their (most) preferred partner. Females from these pairs were alternately assigned to one of two treatments: half of them were allowed to stay with their chosen partner, while the other half were force-paired with the chosen partner of another female from the same aviary. This ensured that, on average, individuals of both treatments were of the same quality, even if assortative pairing for quality had happened due to intra-sexual competition. All pairs were then placed in individual cages for a few months to enforce pair-bonding in the non-chosen pairs (force-pairing is effective in this species if assigned mates are co-housed in a cage for long enough, see “Methods”). After this period in separate cages, pairs were given the opportunity to breed for about five consecutive months (allowing about three successful broods) in communal aviaries, each containing three pairs from each treatment group. This entire procedure was repeated a second time with the same birds (i.e., free choice during a nonbreeding period, force pairing in cages, and breeding in communal aviaries). This was planned a priori to obtain repeated measurements on individuals under different pairing conditions with a large enough sample size to allow the detection of weak effects. For the second breeding period, two-thirds of the pairs from the first breeding period were broken up; individuals chose a new partner and were either assigned to the same or the other treatment. The other third of the pairs were allowed to keep their partner (chosen or non-chosen) from the first breeding period. This allowed us to better control for any effects of pair-bond duration in statistical models comparing chosen and non-chosen pairs, given that pair-bond formation in chosen pairs systematically started earlier (during the free choice period) than in non-chosen pairs (in cage). In total, we monitored behavior and reproductive success of 46 chosen pairs (C) and 38 non-chosen pairs (NC). Measures of reproductive success were based on paternity analyses that included dead embryos, dead chicks, and surviving offspring. Behavior was scored based on direct observations (285 h) and video recordings (1,424 h).

Results

When released into communal breeding aviaries, each of which contained three chosen and three arranged pairs, the proportion of pairs that stayed together differed between treatment groups (C: 46 out of 50 pairs, NC: 38 out of 50 pairs; Fisher’s exact test p = 0.05). This suggests that birds that were force-paired with a partner they did not choose were more reluctant to breed together. However, this differential rate of divorce between the treatment groups is unlikely to induce a bias in our experimental results for the following reasons: (1) Individuals that divorced during one of the breeding periods did not appear to differ in intrinsic quality from individuals that stayed together, as judged from a comparison of reproductive success obtained in the other season when they did not divorce (relative fitness, mean ± standard error [SE]; divorced: 1.06 ± 0.13, n = 23; not divorced: 0.98 ± 0.08, n = 59; general mixed effect model accounting for treatment, birds matched for year, p = 0.58). (2) To induce a bias that is large enough to explain our results, the difference in intrinsic quality between divorcees and the remaining population would need to be unrealistically large (for further details, see simulation in S3 Text).

Only those pairs that remained together were considered for further analyses. Parameter estimates of traits for the two treatments (C versus NC) are given for each general and generalized linear mixed-effect model. Other relevant statistics, as well as the structure of the models, are provided in Table 1 (referred to as “T1-test #”).

thumbnail
Table 1. Treatment effect on breeding parameters and sexual behaviors.

The structure of each model (fixed and random effects) is given, as well as the mean estimates (±SE) of each treatment group. Underlying data for each test can be found in S1 Data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.t001

Total Fitness: Mate Choice for Compatibility?

We calculated relative fitness of individuals as the total number of genetic offspring produced in a given breeding period that reached independence (35 d old), relative to the number produced in the same period by the other individuals in the same aviary. Males of chosen pairs had a 45% higher relative fitness than males of non-chosen pairs (C = 1.16, NC = 0.80, p = 0.03, n = 84 male breeding periods, see T1-1 for model details, Fig 2). Females of chosen pairs had a 30% higher relative fitness than those of non-chosen pairs, but the difference was not significant (C = 1.09, NC = 0.84, p = 0.12, n = 84, T1-2, Fig 2). The difference between the sexes was not significant (interaction between treatment and sex: p = 0.36) and resulted from extra-pair paternity (see below). Thus, on average, individuals from the chosen pairs had a 37% higher fitness. This difference in fitness was not due to differences in pair bond duration between the treatments groups, as this covariate did not correlate with fitness (non-significant trends against the expectation, Table 2: T2-1 and T2-2) and was therefore removed from the models T1-1 and T1-2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Relative fitness estimates (mean ± SE) of males (n = 84) and females (n = 84) from chosen and non-chosen pairs.

Parameter estimates and SE were given by the general linear mixed-effect models T1-1 and T1-2. Underlying data for both tests can be found in S1 Data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.g002

thumbnail
Table 2. Effect of pair bond duration when included as a fixed effect in the models presented in Table 1.

Underlying data for each test can be found in S1 Data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.t002

Fitness Components: Genetic Versus Behavioral Compatibility?

The overall fitness difference observed was not due to differential investment in egg production by the females of the two treatment groups (total number of eggs laid: C = 13.5, NC = 14.4, p = 0.56, n = 84, T1-3). However, non-chosen pairs tended to have a higher proportion of disappeared or buried eggs (C = 12%, NC = 19%, p = 0.07, n = 1172 eggs laid, T1-4), and had significantly more clutches that contained infertile eggs (C = 8%, NC = 23%, p = 0.01, n = 216 clutches, T1-8).

To test the genetic incompatibility hypothesis, we compared the proportion of dead embryos between treatment groups, considering all fertilized and incubated eggs. We only included the genetic eggs of each pair, that is, we excluded all extra-pair young (9% of the eggs), but included eggs that were dumped into the nest of other pairs (13% of the genotyped eggs). Note that removing dumped eggs (potentially suffering higher rate of embryo mortality [65]) from the analysis did not change the conclusions. Furthermore, we only included eggs that were incubated without interruption, excluding those that were buried in the nest material before incubation was completed (based on daily nest checks). The rate of embryo mortality did not differ between chosen and non-chosen pairs (C = 20%, NC = 22%, p = 0.68, n = 707 fertilized eggs, T1-5, Fig 3A).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Embryo (A) and offspring (B) mortality rates (parameter estimates [mean ± SE]) in chosen and non-chosen pairs.

Parameter estimates and SE were given by the generalized linear mixed-effect models T1-5 and T1-6 based on n = 707 fertilized eggs and n = 594 hatched eggs, respectively. Underlying data for both tests can be found in S1 Data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.g003

To test the behavioral compatibility hypothesis, we compared the proportion of dead offspring between treatment groups, considering all hatched eggs in a pair’s nest (including extra-pair offspring and hatchlings from dumped eggs). Offspring mortality was significantly higher when chicks were reared by non-chosen pairs (C = 32%, NC = 52%, p = 0.03, n = 594 hatched eggs, T1-6, Fig 3B). Pair bond duration did not influence this result (T2-6). The probability of survival may also decrease if the offspring is unrelated to one or both of the parents. To check this, we added the status of the offspring (within-pair versus extra-pair young, offspring from dumped versus not dumped egg) into model T1-6. We found that the treatment effect was still significant (p = 0.045), but offspring status was not (mortality of within-pair young = 38%, extra-pair young = 55%, p = 0.15; dumped = 40%, non-dumped = 39%, p = 0.91; underlying data can be found in S1 Data).

Exploratory Analyses: Behavior of Chosen and Non-chosen Pairs

Within-pair courtship behavior.

Males from both treatment groups courted their partner equally often (C = 0.48, NC = 0.52 courtships per hour, p = 0.46, n = 84 male breeding periods, T1-20). Yet, females were significantly less responsive to their partner during courtship if they were paired to a male they had not chosen themselves (C = 0.20, NC = 0.05, p = 0.01, n = 2,503 courtships, T1-15). Within-pair responsiveness was not affected by pair bond duration (T2-15). In non-chosen pairs, 16% of courtships led to copulation, which was not significantly lower than the rate of 20% in chosen pairs (p = 0.28, n = 2,555 courtships, T1-17).

Extra-pair courtship behavior and paternity.

Females of both treatment groups were courted by extra-pair males equally often (C = 0.36, NC = 0.33 courtships per hour, p = 0.75, n = 84, T1-21), after controlling for the fact that females with longer pair bonds were courted significantly less (p = 0.01, T2-21). The response of females towards a courting extra-pair male was mostly negative, independent of the treatment group (C = -0.53, NC = -0.51, p = 0.39, n = 2,752 extra-pair courtships, T1-16), and significantly declined with pair bond duration (T2-16). Females of non-chosen pairs were only non-significantly more likely to engage in extra-pair copulations (C = 6%, NC = 16%, p = 0.13, n = 84 females, T1-18) and to have clutches that contained eggs sired by an extra-pair male (C = 6%, NC = 11%, p = 0.39, n = 245 clutches, T1-9), after controlling for the fact that females with longer pair bonds tended to have fewer extra-pair offspring (p = 0.09, T2-9). These tests for treatment effects on female promiscuity were somewhat inconclusive because relatively large estimated effects in the expected direction were still far from significance, indicating a shortage of statistical power. Males, for their part, had significantly higher extra-pair courtship rates when assigned to a partner (C = 0.20, NC = 0.34 courtships per hour, p = 0.05, n = 84, T1-22), after controlling for the fact that males from pairs with longer pair bonds engaged more in extra-pair courtships (p < 0.001, T2-22). Males of both treatment groups were equally likely to obtain an extra-pair copulation (C = 17%, NC = 13%, p = 0.64, n = 84 courting extra-pair males, T1-19), and males of chosen pairs were non-significantly more successful in siring at least one extra-pair young (C = 32%, NC = 17%, p = 0.16, n = 84, T1-23).

Nest attendance.

Male nest attendance showed a pronounced peak when eggs hatched (S1 Fig). Most of the offspring that died (58%), died within 24–48 h after hatching (i.e., maximally seen once alive on daily nest checks). Thus, we considered nest attendance during the entire offspring rearing period, and also specifically on those days during which one or more offspring hatched in a given nest. Males of non-chosen pairs attended their nest significantly less than those of chosen pairs during the days of hatching (C = 42%, NC = 30%, p = 0.04, n = 556 d of hatching, T1-14), but not during the entire offspring rearing period (C = 22%, NC = 18%, p = 0.24, n = 2,081 d with nestlings, T1-13).

“Pair harmony.”.

To test whether pairs showing more behavioral compatibility before breeding will have higher reproductive success [23], we recorded the occurrence of affiliative versus agonistic behavior, the propensity of individuals to follow their partner, and the distance and synchrony in activity between the members of a pair. These measures, as well as courtship rates and female responsiveness to courtships, were entered into a principal component analysis (PCA). Chosen and non-chosen pairs differed significantly in their PC1 score (C = 0.24, NC = -0.29, p = 0.01, n = 84, S3 Table), with individuals of chosen pairs mainly staying closer together and behaving more synchronously than those of non-chosen pairs (S2 and S3 Tables). PC1 only weakly predicted pair fitness, though the trend was in the expected direction (r = 0.18, n = 84, p = 0.11, S4 Table).

The choice treatment did not have any effect on egg dumping (T1-10), brood size (T1-11), chick mass at day 8 (T1-7), and the interval between clutches (T1-12; see S4 Text).

Discussion

Many studies have attempted to quantify the benefits of mate choice [27,3546,4850,66,67], but only a few have quantified the fitness benefits of mate choice for compatibility while excluding quality benefits (see e.g. [46] and [50]) (Fig 1). Our experimental design allowed us to circumvent the potentially confounding effect of mate quality by comparing pairs of individuals that chose each other with pairs that were composed of random individuals who did not choose each other, but had both been chosen by another individual. Pairs that formed through free mate choice had a 37% higher fitness than pairs that were “forced” experimentally (Fig 2). This suggests that it is unlikely that the between-individual disagreement about mate attractiveness simply reflects indecision or measurement error. Our results suggest instead that individual-specific mate preferences lead to significant fitness consequences.

Our study system, furthermore, allowed us to disentangle direct (behavioral) benefits of mate choice from indirect (genetic) benefits (Fig 1). Chosen pairs, compared to arranged ones, had a 38% lower rate of offspring mortality (Fig 3B). Under the assumption that offspring mortality systematically depends on parental behavior, this result supports the hypothesis of mate choice for behavioral compatibility. Ideally, our experiment should be repeated while cross-fostering eggs to exclude confounding factors. Indeed, our conclusions depend on the generalizability of the results from our previous study (S1 Text). The finding that offspring mortality after hatching primarily depends on the rearing parents and not on the genetic parents (S1 Table) can likely be generalized from our previous cross-fostering experiment to this study; in both studies, many offspring apparently died from starvation, and an offspring that is not fed will die irrespective of its genetic quality. Finally, chosen and arranged pairs had an equal rate of embryo mortality (Fig 3A). Given that embryo mortality primarily depends on the genetic parents and less on the incubating parents (S1 Table), this result argues against the hypothesis of mate choice for genetic compatibility. At least, our results suggest that individuals did not select a partner with whom they would have minimized the rate of embryo mortality.

Several earlier experimental studies favored the genetic compatibility hypothesis based on the observation that offspring from “free-choice” pairs had a higher viability than those from “forced” pairs [3537,40,43,46,66]. However, in these experiments females were forced to mate with random males from the population or with non-preferred males, some of which may have been of lower absolute quality (but see [46]). Hence, the previously observed effects on offspring viability may be explained by differences in both genetic quality and compatibility. In general, mate choice for genetic compatibility may not easily evolve, because it requires that the incompatibility-causing loci are tightly linked (e.g., via pleiotropy) to a detectable phenotype and to a mechanism ensuring the appropriate assortative or disassortative preference [68]. At least in zebra finches, such a complex adaptation that would allow them to minimize embryo mortality by choosing a genetically compatible partner, does not seem to exist (this study). Similarly, inbreeding avoidance is absent in this species when birds can only judge genetic similarity per se [61] (although it does take place when siblings are familiar with each other [69]).

Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that behavioral compatibility between the pair members leads to benefits of mate choice. This could come about through different mechanisms: the emerging behaviors of a pair in terms of coordination or complementarity [23,24,2729], and/or the individual-specific stimulation of a partner’s sensory system leading to a greater investment in reproduction [3234]. Currently it is unclear which of these factors leads to the observed variation in parental care compatibility, and it is also unclear to what extent there is a genetic basis for this variation in compatibility. In the following, we discuss our exploratory analyses on fitness components and behaviors of “free-choice” and “forced” pairs, to provide testable ideas about how such behavioral compatibility benefits could arise.

We found that non-chosen pairs (1) more often had clutches with infertile eggs, (2) had more offspring dying at an early stage (presumably from starvation), and (3) tended to have more eggs that disappeared (presumably due to poorer care and nest defense). These effects on components of fitness may be due to differences in the behavior of chosen and non-chosen pairs.

The most prominent behavioral differences were that (a) females with assigned partners responded less positively to within-pair courtship and they tended to copulate less frequently with their partner, and (b) males with assigned partners showed poorer nest attendance during the egg hatching period. The females’ reduced tendency to participate in within-pair courtship and copulation when in a “forced” pair may explain the higher incidence of infertile eggs. Indeed, in a previous experiment in which continuous video recording allowed us to witness about 80% of all copulations over a 4-mo period (partly reported in [53]) we found that the probability of laying an infertile egg declined significantly with the number of copulations witnessed during the 10 d prior to egg laying (p = 0.04, n = 376 eggs laid by 31 females, estimates: 27% infertile at 0 copulations versus 15% infertile at the median of 5 copulations; underlying data can be found in S1 Data). Alternatively, apparently infertile eggs may in fact represent cases of very early embryo mortality. This seems unlikely because egg fertility scores in zebra finches were tightly linked to the number of sperm that reached the egg [70]. Likewise, the lower nest attendance during hatching by males in non-chosen pairs could indicate a reduced motivation to care for the young or defend the nest when in a forced partnership, leading to greater offspring mortality and egg loss. Consequently, the results of these exploratory analyses further support the behavioral compatibility hypothesis.

If males and females in “forced” pairs indeed invest less in reproduction (copulation or care), as our results suggest, the question remains why. Reduced investment by members of “forced” pairs could be a long-term effect of a single stressful event (trauma), namely the loss of the chosen partner (an event that could also happen in the wild due to predation [71]). This explanation seems unlikely, however, because fitness was affected by the treatment per se and not by the number of partner losses experienced by an individual (see scheme in “Methods”) when both factors were fitted within one model (males: treatment p = 0.02, number of mate losses p = 0.63; females: treatment p = 0.06, number of mate losses p = 0.35). Alternatively, being forced to breed with a non-preferred partner (unlikely to occur in the wild) might cause chronic stress. Being chronically stressed when paired to a specific partner A but not when paired to partner B would be part of the “phenotypic incompatibility” phenomenon.

Our score of “pair harmony,” which was based on affiliative and sexual behaviors, as well as behavioral synchrony and the tendency to reunite, did not significantly correlate with pair fitness. A study on zebra finches in the wild reported that behavioral synchrony was associated with brood size [25], but further experimental work suggested that variation in synchrony might have been the consequence and not the cause of variation in reproductive success [72].

Evidence supporting the idea that pair coordination is important mainly comes from studies showing an increase in breeding success with pair bond duration ([2729,73,74] but see [75]). We specifically designed our experiment to create variation in pair-bond duration (pairs stayed together for one or two breeding periods). However, this covariate did not have an effect on any of the fitness components (mostly showing non-significant trends opposite to expectation, Table 2) and was therefore removed from most final models. This suggests that behavioral compatibility (with synergistic effects on fitness) did not increase with pair bond duration.

The only traits that were affected by pair-bond duration were those related to extra-pair behavior (Table 2): females responded less positively to extra-pair courtships and received fewer extra-pair courtships with increasing pair-bond duration. In contrast, male courtship rate towards extra-pair females increased with pair-bond duration. In other words, it seems that females decreased and males increased their promiscuous behavior.

It has been suggested that individuals choose each other based on their respective personality, which would determine their behavioral compatibility [22]. Individuals that show similar behavioral types, or similar plasticity (and therefore predictability), could be better at negotiating or coordinating their actions, and could therefore have reduced conflicts over parental care and higher reproductive success ([26,76,77] but see [78]). So far, besides observational studies [76,79,80], only two experiments (both conducted on zebra finches) aimed at testing this hypothesis, and none of them found consistent evidence for pair combination effects on rearing success, based on any of the personality traits measured [26,78]. We did not measure any personality traits of individuals prior to the experiment, because we did not have clear a priori predictions about the advantages of being behaviorally similar. Instead, we scored the synchrony of activities during breeding, but this did not differ between treatment groups (see S4 Text).

Although an effect of lack of coordination between pair members cannot be excluded, our exploratory analyses suggest a reduced investment or commitment in individuals of “forced” pairs (lower female within-pair responsiveness, higher male extra-pair courtship rate, lower male nest attendance). Previous experimental work on zebra finches shows that the amount of male singing activity can affect egg quality [33]. More generally, courtship and other affiliative behaviors, which may occur more frequently in chosen pairs, may affect the level of reciprocal stimulation [32,81,82]. Earlier studies that favored the genetic compatibility hypothesis cannot rule out that the treatment (chosen versus non-chosen pairs) affected maternal investment (e.g., egg quality) with potential effects on offspring viability [3537,43,46]. Artificial insemination would be needed to experimentally demonstrate that higher offspring viability arises from genetic compatibility and not from maternal (e.g., egg nutrients) or paternal effects (e.g., sperm allocation) following greater stimulation by a preferred partner (see e.g., [5]).

If forced pairs reduced their investment in breeding together, as our analyses suggest, the question remains whether this behavior is adaptive. Reduced investment in current reproduction could be adaptive, if it saves resources for future reproduction with a better (preferred) partner. However, this explanation seems unlikely for a species such as the zebra finch, because life-long monogamy largely precludes breeding with a different partner in the future [52]. Moreover, in a follow-up experiment consisting of a third breeding season where all individuals could freely choose their mate, individuals could not compensate for the lower fitness previously obtained with a non-chosen partner (see S6 Text). Therefore, the reduced investment in breeding by members of non-chosen pairs could be maladaptive, either because this never occurs under natural conditions (because individuals are never forced to mate or breed with a particular partner), or because some constraints limit the adaptive behavioral flexibility of the animals.

To conclude, chosen pairs had significantly higher fitness than forced pairs, apparently due to behavioral rather than genetic compatibility effects. The mechanisms behind such behavioral compatibility, in terms of willingness or ability to cooperate with certain individuals and in terms of coordination between partners need further study, in particular in the context of offspring provisioning. In humans, some studies suggest that individuals are more satisfied, more committed, and less likely to engage in domestic violence, when involved in a love-based rather than an arranged marriage ([83,84], but see [85]). The challenge there is also to find out whether stable and happy marriages result from motivation to cooperate (and to identify what stimulates such feelings, see [8689]), or from congruence in terms of partners’ intrinsic behavioral types [90].

Methods

The study was approved by the Animal Care and Ethics Committee of the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology.

Design

A scheme of the design with its timeline is depicted in Fig 4. All experimental birds hatched in the summer of 2011 in large semi-outdoor aviaries. The origin of the birds (population #4 in [91]), and rearing and housing conditions have been described in detail elsewhere [60]. This population has been derived from the wild only about ten generations ago [91]. Shortly after independence (age 45 d), individuals were put into eight mixed-sex peer-groups of ten males and ten females. When birds reached sexual maturity (100 d old) they were color-banded, and peer-groups were joined two by two (yielding four groups, each allowing 20 possible pairs to form). Sixty-six pairs were identified during ad libitum observations in the winter of 2011–2012. Mid-April 2012, half of the identified pairs were randomly assigned to the treatment group NC (in which all birds are assigned the partner of someone else: “non-chosen”), while the remaining pairs went to the treatment group C (in which birds are allowed to stay with their chosen partner). In order to induce pair formation in the randomly created pairs of the NC group, these pairs were put into individual cages for a period of two months and were allowed to lay one clutch. Pairs from the C group also went to such cages and were allowed to lay one clutch in order to standardize all experiences apart from the re-pairing. On the 21st of May, three pairs of each treatment group (chosen randomly but excluding the initial chosen partners of individuals of non-chosen pairs) were put into a breeding aviary (10 replicates, 60 pairs in total). Both members of each pair had been previously color-banded on both legs with one random color out of six (dark blue, light blue, black, yellow, orange, white), so that a pair would be unmistakably identifiable in its aviary. Forty-five pairs (26 C, 19 NC) did not divorce and were considered for the analyses. After one week of intensive focal pair observations, we introduced nest material, and checked nests daily until 21 August, when the experiment was stopped and newly laid eggs were replaced by dummy eggs, but pairs were still allowed to raise all offspring from eggs laid before that.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Experimental design and timeline.

Chosen pairs (C, n = 46, filled red hearts) resulted from free choice. Non-chosen pairs (NC, n = 38, broken yellow hearts) resulted from force-pairing (by being put together in a cage; experimental stage 2) between individuals that expressed a choice (during experimental stage 1), but who were separated from their initial chosen partner (event symbolized by yellow lightning). Fitness of all pairs was measured during experimental stage 3. The follow-up study (S6 Text) took place in spring 2014. All experimental birds had hatched in the summer of 2011.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.g004

In October 2012, once all offspring had reached independence, we assigned treatment groups for the second breeding season. First, we randomly selected eight pairs from each treatment group (among the 26 C and 19 NC that were available) that were allowed to stay with their partner throughout the second season. In this way, we could better separate the effects of choice treatment from the potential effects of pair-bond duration. These 16 pairs and all other adults (remaining C, NC, and previously divorced birds) were put into one of two big flocks, to allow a second round of choosing a partner. Each group contained 20 widowed males (i.e., their former breeding partner was in the other group), 20 widowed females, and eight established pairs. As a result, each widowed female could choose a new partner among a set of 20 new males, which never included her previous breeding partner (but for half of the females from non-chosen pairs it did include again the initially chosen mate because this could not be avoided). In December 2012, after pair identification and random assignment to treatment group (without regard to previous treatment), pairs were put into cages for six months and allowed to lay two clutches. The longer period of force-pairing in cages resulted in a lower divorce rate compared to the previous season (only one pair of each treatment divorced). On 21 May 2013, pairs were put again into breeding aviaries and allowed to breed as in the previous year. Of 52 pairs identified in the winter groups, 42 (21 of each treatment group, across seven aviaries) contributed to the second breeding period (12 birds died accidentally because food dispensers were blocked for 2 d in early March 2013). The design itself and the accidental food shortage may have led to selection of the highest quality individuals. Although it did not induce bias (selection was independent of treatment group), it can result in an underestimation of the real fitness benefits of mate choice. Furthermore, one member of a pair died for unknown reasons (and its partner was removed) within the first week of each breeding period (1 C in 2012 and 1 NC in 2013), and these two pairs were excluded from the analyses.

Breeding Monitoring

Each aviary contained 7 nest boxes. Every morning, all nests were checked, the individual(s) attending the nest identified, and the fate of each egg and each offspring noted. Unhatched eggs were opened when neglected by the parents (for instance, after offspring had fledged) and embryos were collected for parentage analysis (using 11 microsatellite markers, following [92] and [93]). For the same purpose, small (~10 μl) blood samples were taken from 8–10 d old offspring, or tissue samples if they died earlier. Of 1,434 eggs laid by all birds including divorcees, 28% (n = 402) could not be assigned through parentage analysis, and were assigned to the social pair that attended the nest. These eggs included apparently infertile eggs (5.6%, n = 80), and eggs that were buried in the nest and did not develop (typically after a nest take-over by another pair) or disappeared (presumably they broke and were eaten by the birds) (21.6%, n = 310), as well as eight dead embryos and four dead hatchlings that yielded bad DNA samples. Relative fitness of an individual was calculated as the total number of genetic offspring produced in a given breeding period that reached independence (age 35 d) divided by the average number of offspring produced by all same-sex individuals of the same aviary that did not divorce.

Behavioral Observations

Each aviary was equipped with a dome camera set to record different aviary positions during each day of the week. During 3 d, we filmed an artificial tree, on which 69% of all courtships took place (calculated from direct observations, described below). For one day, we recorded each of the two sets of nest boxes, and for 2 d, a set of perches on which individuals often allopreen. We analyzed the first hour of each day, when copulations are most frequent [53]. In all pairs considered for the analyses (those that did not divorce), we recorded 1,942 within-pair (WP) and 2,999 extra-pair (EP) courtships (in the latter, a divorced female or male may have been the extra-pair partner). For each courtship, we scored female responsiveness as follows: threat or aggression toward the male (−1), flying away (−0.5), mixed or ambiguous signs (0), courtship hopping and beak wiping (+0.5), and copulation solicitation (+1), and noted whether it led to a successful copulation.

We also conducted direct observations, following a protocol inspired by studies on cockatiels, Nymphicus hollandicus [21,23,94]. Observations were carried out both in the pre-breeding period (first week after release into aviaries before nesting material was added) and during the entire breeding period, to test whether pairs with greater behavioral compatibility before breeding (as in [23]), or during breeding activities, would have greater reproductive success. The observer stood behind a one-way glass window (built into each aviary door) and carried out focal-pair watches by monitoring a pair for 3 min. During these watches we observed 613 WP and 800 EP courtships. We noted their location and whether they led to a successful copulation. For a subset of 561 WP and 782 EP courtships, we also scored female responsiveness, as described above. During focal-pair watches, we also recorded whether within-pair allopreening or aggression occurred during the 3 min period (“yes” or “no”). Every 30 s, we recorded the distance between the partners and their activity. Distance was averaged for each 3 min watch. Activities were split into nine categories: feeding, cleaning, nesting or parental behavior (nest building or attendance, and feeding of fledglings), sleeping, sitting, involved in aggression, involved in courtship, flying, and “other.” We defined pair synchrony as the sum of the observations in which both partners engaged in the same activity (range 0–6). For each pair member, we also recorded all occurrences of an individual flying away from or back towards (<50 cm) its partner (e.g., female flying away: Faway, male flying back: Mback). From those counts, we calculated the tendency of the pair to reunite: (Σ Fback + Σ Mback) / (Σ Faway + Σ Maway), and a mate guarding index: (Σ FawayΣ Fback) − (Σ MawayΣ Mback). The latter is positive in case of male mate guarding, and negative for female mate guarding. The six pairs in an aviary were watched successively in a randomized predetermined order, and the time of observation of each aviary was randomized over the course of each day (i.e., from sunrise to sunset). In 2012, pairs were watched 9–13 times (median = 11) in the pre-breeding period, and 37–39 times (median = 38) during the breeding period. In 2013, 16–21 focal watches (median = 21) per pair were performed during pre-breeding, and 68–70 (median = 69) during breeding. For each pair, all measures were averaged for all focal watches separately for the pre-breeding and breeding period, because these periods were analyzed separately as planned a priori (see Results and S4 Text). Male courtship rates (WP and EP courtships per hour) and best linear unbiased predictors (BLUPs, i.e., random effect estimates) of female responsiveness (to WP and EP courtships) were also calculated (see S5 Text) for both periods and included in a principal component analysis (PCA). All observations were done blind to the treatment of the birds.

Data Analyses

All statistical tests were conducted in R [95]. General and generalized mixed-effect models were performed with the “lmer” and “glmer” function of the lme4 package [96] and the PCAs with the “principal” function of the psych package [97]. All fixed effects were chosen a priori by considering (a) their biological relevance (e.g., hatching order when looking at offspring mortality), (b) their mathematical relevance (e.g., clutch size when looking at the presence of infertile eggs in a clutch), (c) the experimental design (e.g., year), and (d) consistency with previously published models (e.g., how to model the fertile period when looking at female responsiveness). P-values for general mixed effect models (lmer) were obtained from model comparison (with and without the explanatory variable) with the function anova in R; p-values for generalized mixed effect models (glmer) were taken from the model output (calculated from z-values).

Supporting Information

S1 Data. Excel spreadsheet containing, in separate sheets, the underlying numerical data for each test presented in Tables 1 and 2, S1 Table, part A of S3 and S4 Tables, part B of S3 and S4 Tables, S6 Text Males and Females, S1 Fig, and for the analysis presented in the discussion (the relationship between egg fertility and the number of observed copulations).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s001

(XLSX)

S1 Fig. Proportion of daily nest checks in which the female, the male, or both pair members simultaneously attended their nest while having an active clutch or a brood.

The values for joint nest attendance are included in the values for females and males. Day 1 is the start of incubation. In zebra finches, incubation lasts on average 12 d, and offspring fledge 15–20 d after hatching.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s002

(TIF)

S1 Table. Causes of embryo and offspring mortality in cross-fostered eggs shown as variance component estimates.

The analysis is described in S1 Text. Results are from a domesticated population (#18 in [45]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s003

(PDF)

S2 Table. Loadings of behavioral variables on PC1 for the pre-breeding (A), and the breeding period (B).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s004

(PDF)

S3 Table. Results of linear models investigating the effect of the treatment on PC1 scores and on each variable included in the PCA separately, for the pre-breeding (A) and the breeding period (B).

For all tests, n = 84 pairs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s005

(PDF)

S4 Table. Results of linear models investigating the effect of PC1, and each variable included in the PCA separately, on the relative fitness achieved by the pairs.

Analyses for the pre-breeding (A) and the breeding period (B) are shown. For all tests, n = 84 pairs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s006

(PDF)

S1 Text. Causes of offspring and embryo mortality in zebra finches: egg cross-fostering experiments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s007

(PDF)

S2 Text. Pilot study on the assessment of mating preferences.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s008

(PDF)

S3 Text. Analysis and discussion of the differential rates of divorce between chosen and non-chosen pairs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s009

(PDF)

S4 Text. Treatment effects on other fitness-related traits and behaviors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s010

(PDF)

S5 Text. Details on behavioral variables used in the PCA.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s011

(PDF)

S6 Text. Follow-up experiment: opportunity to compensate later in life.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248.s012

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to J. Schreiber, T. Aronson, and A. Edme for help with breeding monitoring and live observations; M. Schneider for molecular work; and K. Martin for video analyses. M.I. was part of the International Max Planck Research School for Organismal Biology during the course of this project.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MI BK WF. Performed the experiments: MI. Analyzed the data: MI WF. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MI BK WF. Wrote the paper: MI BK WF.

References

  1. 1. Gowaty PA, Anderson WW, Bluhm CK, Drickamer LC, Kim YK, Moore AJ. (2007) The hypothesis of reproductive compensation and its assumptions about mate preferences and offspring viability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 15023–15027. pmid:17848509
  2. 2. Ryan KK, Altmann J (2001) Selection for male choice based primarily on mate compatibility in the oldfield mouse, Peromyscus polionotus rhoadsi. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50: 436–440.
  3. 3. Petrie M (1994) Improved growth and survival of offspring of peacocks with more elaborate trains. Nature 371: 598–599.
  4. 4. Head ML, Hunt J, Jennions MD, Brooks R (2005) The indirect benefits of mating with attractive males outweigh the direct costs. PLoS Biology 3: 289–294.
  5. 5. Parker TH (2003) Genetic benefits of mate choice separated from differential maternal investment in red junglefowl (Gallus gallus). Evolution 57: 2157–2165. pmid:14575335
  6. 6. Reynolds JD, Gross MR (1992) Female mate preference enhances offspring growth and reproduction in a fish, Poecilia reticulata. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 250: 57–62.
  7. 7. Simmons LW (1987) Female choice contributes to offspring fitness in the field cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus (De Geer). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 21: 313–321.
  8. 8. Møller AP, Jennions MD (2001) How important are direct fitness benefits of sexual selection? Naturwissenschaften 88: 401–415. pmid:11729807
  9. 9. Maklakov AA, Arnqvist G (2009) Testing for direct and indirect effects of mate choice by manipulating female choosiness. Current Biology 19: 1903–1906. pmid:19853448
  10. 10. Kotiaho JS, Puurtinen M (2007) Mate choice for indirect genetic benefits: scrutiny of the current paradigm. Functional Ecology 21: 638–644.
  11. 11. Neff BD, Pitcher TE (2005) Genetic quality and sexual selection: an integrated framework for good genes and compatible genes. Molecular Ecology 14: 19–38. pmid:15643948
  12. 12. Cutrera AP, Fanjul MS, Zenuto RR (2012) Females prefer good genes: MHC-associated mate choice in wild and captive tuco-tucos. Animal Behaviour 83: 847–856.
  13. 13. Mays HL, Hill GE (2004) Choosing mates: good genes versus genes that are a good fit. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19: 554–559.
  14. 14. Brooks R, Endler JA (2001) Female guppies agree to differ: Phenotypic and genetic variation in mate-choice behavior and the consequences for sexual selection. Evolution 55: 1644–1655. pmid:11580024
  15. 15. Forstmeier W, Birkhead TR (2004) Repeatability of mate choice in the zebra finch: consistency within and between females. Animal Behaviour 68: 1017–1028.
  16. 16. Godin JGJ, Dugatkin LA (1995) Variability and repeatability of female mating preference in the guppy. Animal Behaviour 49: 1427–1433.
  17. 17. Jennions MD, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences. Biological Reviews 72: 283–327. pmid:9155244
  18. 18. Lehtonen TK, Lindström K (2008) Repeatability of mating preferences in the sand goby. Animal Behaviour 75: 55–61.
  19. 19. Widemo F, Saether SA (1999) Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: causes and consequences of variation in mating preferences. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 14: 26–31.
  20. 20. Delesalle VA (1986) Division of parental care and reproductive success in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Behavioural Processes 12: 1–22. pmid:24924533
  21. 21. Spoon TR, Millam JR, Owings DH (2007) Behavioural compatibility, extrapair copulation and mate switching in a socially monogamous parrot. Animal Behaviour 73: 815–824.
  22. 22. Schuett W, Tregenza T, Dall SRX (2010) Sexual selection and animal personality. Biological Reviews 85: 217–246. pmid:19922534
  23. 23. Spoon TR, Millam JR, Owings DH (2006) The importance of mate behavioural compatibility in parenting and reproductive success by cockatiels, Nymphicus hollandicus. Animal Behaviour 71: 315–326.
  24. 24. Fox RA, Millam JR (2014) Personality traits of pair members predict pair compatibility and reproductive success in a socially monogamous parrot breeding in captivity. Zoo Biology 33: 166–172. pmid:24619540
  25. 25. Mariette MM, Griffith SC (2012) Nest visit synchrony is high and correlates with reproductive success in the wild zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata. Journal of Avian Biology 43: 131–140.
  26. 26. Schuett W, Dall SRX, Royle NJ (2011) Pairs of zebra finches with similar 'personalities' make better parents. Animal Behaviour 81: 609–618.
  27. 27. Griggio M, Hoi H (2011) An experiment on the function of the long-term pair bond period in the socially monogamous bearded reedling. Animal Behaviour 82: 1329–1335.
  28. 28. Sánchez-Macouzet O, Rodriguez C, Drummond H (2014) Better stay together: pair bond duration increases individual fitness independent of age-related variation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 281.
  29. 29. van de Pol M, Heg D, Bruinzeel LW, Kuijper B, Verhulst S (2006) Experimental evidence for a causal effect of pair-bond duration on reproductive performance in oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus). Behavioral Ecology 17: 982–991.
  30. 30. Mainwaring MC, Griffith SC (2013) Looking after your partner: sentinel behaviour in a socially monogamous bird. PeerJ 1: e83–e83. pmid:23761856
  31. 31. Crespi BJ (1989) Causes of assortative mating in arthropods. Animal Behaviour 38: 980–1000.
  32. 32. Servedio MR, Price TD, Lande R (2013) Evolution of displays within the pair bond. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 280.
  33. 33. Bolund E, Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W (2012) Singing activity stimulates partner reproductive investment rather than increasing paternity success in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66: 975–984.
  34. 34. Arak A, Enquist M (1993) Hidden preferences and the evolution of signals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 340: 207–213.
  35. 35. Anderson WW, Kim YK, Gowaty PA (2007) Experimental constraints on mate preferences in Drosophila pseudoobscura decrease offspring viability and fitness of mated pairs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 4484–4488. pmid:17360550
  36. 36. Drickamer LC, Gowaty PA, Holmes CM (2000) Free female mate choice in house mice affects reproductive success and offspring viability and performance. Animal Behaviour 59: 371–378. pmid:10675259
  37. 37. Gowaty PA, Drickamer LC, Schmid-Holmes S (2003) Male house mice produce fewer offspring with lower viability and poorer performance when mated with females they do not prefer. Animal Behaviour 65: 95–103.
  38. 38. Moore AJ, Gowaty PA, Wallin WG, Moore PJ (2001) Sexual conflict and the evolution of female mate choice and male social dominance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 268: 517–523.
  39. 39. Sandvik M, Rosenqvist G, Berglund A (2000) Male and female mate choice affects offspring quality in a sex-role-reversed pipefish. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 267: 2151–2155.
  40. 40. Raveh S, Sutalo S, Thonhauser KE, Thoss M, Hettyey A, Winkelser F, et al. (2014) Female partner preferences enhance offspring ability to survive an infection. BMC Evolutionary Biology 14.
  41. 41. Pogany A, Szurovecz Z, Vincze E, Barta Z, Szekely T (2014) Mate preference does not influence reproductive motivation and parental cooperation in female zebra finches. Behaviour 151: 1885–1901.
  42. 42. Martin MS, Shepherdson DJ (2012) Role of familiarity and preference in reproductive success in ex situ breeding programs. Conservation Biology 26: 649–656. pmid:22809353
  43. 43. Drickamer LC, Gowaty PA, Wagner DM (2003) Free mutual mate preferences in house mice affect reproductive success and offspring performance. Animal Behaviour 65: 105–114.
  44. 44. Bottoni L, Massa R, Lea RW, Sharp PJ (1993) Mate choice and reproductive success in the red-legged partridge (Alectoris rufa). Hormones and Behavior 27: 308–317. pmid:8225255
  45. 45. Massa R, Galanti V, Bottoni L (1996) Mate choice and reproductive success in the domesticated budgerigar, Melopsittacus undulatus. Italian Journal of Zoology 63: 243–246.
  46. 46. Bluhm CK, Gowaty PA (2004) Social constraints on female mate preferences in mallards, Anas platyrhynchos, decrease offspring viability and mother productivity. Animal Behaviour 68: 977–983.
  47. 47. Edward DA, Chapman T (2012) Measuring the fitness benefits of male mate choice in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution 66: 2646–2653. pmid:22834761
  48. 48. Gleason ED, Holschbach MA, Marler CA (2012) Compatibility drives female preference and reproductive success in the monogamous California mouse (Peromyscus californicus) more strongly than male testosterone measures. Hormones and Behavior 61: 100–107. pmid:22101260
  49. 49. Lehtonen TK, Lindström K (2007) Mate compatibility, parental allocation and fitness consequences of mate choice in the sand goby Pomatoschistus minutus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 1581–1588.
  50. 50. Wedell N, Tregenza T (1999) Successful fathers sire successful sons. Evolution 53: 620–625.
  51. 51. Kempenaers B (2007) Mate choice and genetic quality: A review of the heterozygosity theory. Advances in the Study of Behavior 37: 189–278.
  52. 52. Zann RA (1996) Zebra Finch: A synthesis of field and laboratory studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  53. 53. Forstmeier W, Martin K, Bolund E, Schielzeth H, Kempenaers B (2011) Female extrapair mating behavior can evolve via indirect selection on males. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 10608–10613. pmid:21670288
  54. 54. Riebel K (2009) Song and female mate choice in zebra finches: A review. Advances in the Study of Behavior. San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press Inc. pp. 197–238.
  55. 55. Simons MJP, Verhulst S (2011) Zebra finch females prefer males with redder bills independent of song rate-a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology 22: 755–762.
  56. 56. Schielzeth H, Bolund E, Forstmeier W (2010) Heritability of and early environment effects on variation in mating preferences. Evolution 64: 998–1006. pmid:19895552
  57. 57. Wynn SE, Price T (1993) Male and female choice in Zebra Finches. Auk 110: 635–638.
  58. 58. Bolund E, Martin K, Kempenaers B, Forstmeier W (2010) Inbreeding depression of sexually selected traits and attractiveness in the zebra finch. Animal Behaviour 79: 947–955.
  59. 59. Griffith SC, Pryke SR, Mariette M (2008) Use of nest-boxes by the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata): implications for reproductive success and research. Emu 108: 311–319.
  60. 60. Ihle M, Kempenaers B, Forstmeier W (2013) Does hatching failure breed infidelity? Behavioral Ecology 24: 119–127.
  61. 61. Schielzeth H, Burger C, Bolund E, Forstmeier W (2008) Assortative versus disassortative mating preferences of female zebra finches based on self-referent phenotype matching. Animal Behaviour 76: 1927–1934.
  62. 62. Schielzeth H, Burger C, Bolund E, Forstmeier W (2008) Sexual imprinting on continuous variation: do female zebra finches prefer or avoid unfamiliar sons of their foster parents? Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21: 1274–1280. pmid:18624884
  63. 63. Dziminski MA, Roberts JD, Simmons LW (2008) Fitness consequences of parental compatibility in the frog Crinia georgiana. Evolution 62: 879–886. pmid:18208566
  64. 64. Rodríguez-Muñoz R, Tregenza T (2009) Genetic compatibility and hatching success in the sea lamprey (Petromyzon marinus). Biology Letters 5: 286–288. pmid:19049954
  65. 65. Schielzeth H, Bolund E (2010) Patterns of conspecific brood parasitism in zebra finches. Animal Behaviour 79: 1329–1337.
  66. 66. Bluhm CK, Gowaty PA (2004) Reproductive compensation for offspring viability deficits by female mallards, Anas platyrhynchos. Animal Behaviour 68: 985–992.
  67. 67. Moore AJ, Gowaty PA, Moore PJ (2003) Females avoid manipulative males and live longer. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16: 523–530. pmid:14635853
  68. 68. Tregenza T, Wedell N (2000) Genetic compatibility, mate choice and patterns of parentage: Invited review. Molecular Ecology 9: 1013–1027. pmid:10964221
  69. 69. Ihle M, Forstmeier W (2013) Revisiting the evidence for inbreeding avoidance in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology 24: 1356–1362.
  70. 70. Birkhead TR, Fletcher F (1998) Sperm transport in the reproductive tract of female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 114: 141–145. pmid:9875166
  71. 71. Remage-Healey L, Adkins-Regan E, Romero LM (2003) Behavioral and adrenocortical responses to mate separation and reunion in the zebra finch. Hormones and Behavior 43: 108–114. pmid:12614640
  72. 72. Mariette MM, Griffith SC (2015) The adaptive significance of provisioning and foraging coordination between breeding partners. American Naturalist 185: 270–280. pmid:25616144
  73. 73. Black JM (2001) Fitness consequences of long-term pair bonds in barnacle geese: monogamy in the extreme. Behavioral Ecology 12: 640–645.
  74. 74. Ens BJ, Choudhury S, Black JM (1996) Mate fidelity and divorce in monogamous birds. In: Black JM, editor. Partnerships in birds: The study of monogamy: Oxford Ornithology Series. pp. 344–395.
  75. 75. Naves LC, Cam E, Monnat JY (2007) Pair duration, breeding success and divorce in a long-lived seabird: benefits of mate familiarity? Animal Behaviour 73: 433–444.
  76. 76. Both C, Dingemanse NJ, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM (2005) Pairs of extreme avian personalities have highest reproductive success. Journal of Animal Ecology 74: 667–674.
  77. 77. Royle NJ, Schuett W, Dall SRX (2010) Behavioral consistency and the resolution of sexual conflict over parental investment. Behavioral Ecology 21: 1125–1130.
  78. 78. Schielzeth H, Bolund E, Kempenaers B, Forstmeier W (2011) Quantitative genetics and fitness consequences of neophilia in zebra finches. Behavioral Ecology 22: 126–134.
  79. 79. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM (2004) Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271: 847–852.
  80. 80. Gabriel PO, Black JM (2012) Behavioural syndromes, partner compatibility and reproductive performance in Steller's jays. Ethology 118: 76–86.
  81. 81. Erickson CJ, Lehrman DS (1964) Effect of castration of male ring doves upon ovarian activity of females. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 58: 164–&. pmid:14215384
  82. 82. Wachtmeister CA (2001) Display in monogamous pairs: a review of empirical data and evolutionary explanations. Animal Behaviour 61: 861–868.
  83. 83. Xu XH, Whyte MK (1990) Love matches and arranged marriages—A Chinese replication. Journal of Marriage and the Family 52: 709–722.
  84. 84. Sahin NH, Timur S, Ergin AB, Taspinar A, Balkaya NA, Cubukcu S (2010) Childhood trauma, type of marriage and self-esteem as correlates of domestic violence in married women in Turkey. Journal of Family Violence 25: 661–668.
  85. 85. Regan PC, Lakhanpal S, Anguiano C (2012) Relationship outcomes in Indian-American love-based and arranged marriages. Psychological Reports 110: 915–924. pmid:22897093
  86. 86. Asendorpf JB, Penke L, Back MD (2011) From dating to mating and relating: predictors of initial and long-term outcomes of speed-dating in a community sample. European Journal of Personality 25: 16–30.
  87. 87. Honekopp J (2006) Once more: Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Relative contributions of private and shared taste to judgments of facial attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance 32: 199–209. pmid:16634665
  88. 88. Meltzer AL, McNulty JK (2014) "Tell me I'm sexy … and otherwise valuable": Body valuation and relationship satisfaction. Personal Relationships 21: 68–87. pmid:24683309
  89. 89. Todd PM, Penke L, Fasolo B, Lenton AP (2007) Different cognitive processes underlie human mate choices and mate preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 15011–15016. pmid:17827279
  90. 90. Rammstedt B, Spinath FM, Richter D, Schupp J (2013) Partnership longevity and personality congruence in couples. Personality and Individual Differences 54: 832–835.
  91. 91. Forstmeier W, Segelbacher G, Mueller JC, Kempenaers B (2007) Genetic variation and differentiation in captive and wild zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Molecular Ecology 16: 4039–4050. pmid:17894758
  92. 92. Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H, Schneider M, Kempenaers B (2007) Development of polymorphic microsatellite markers for the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Molecular Ecology Notes 7: 1026–1028.
  93. 93. Forstmeier W, Mueller JC, Kempenaers B (2010) A polymorphism in the oestrogen receptor gene explains covariance between digit ratio and mating behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 277: 3353–3361.
  94. 94. Spoon TR, Millam JR, Owings DH (2004) Variation in the stability of cockatiel (Nymphicus hollandicus) pair relationships: The roles of males, females, and mate compatibility. Behaviour 141: 1211–1234.
  95. 95. R Core Team (2015) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/.
  96. 96. Bates D, Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker S. (2014) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.0–6. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
  97. 97. Revelle W (2014) psych: procedures for personality and psychological research, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA. R package version 1.4.3.http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych