Male 11-ketotestosterone levels change as a result of being watched in Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2005.12.023Get rights and content

Abstract

This study investigated the effects of nesting status and the presence of an audience on 11-ketotestosterone (11KT) levels in male Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Prior studies have demonstrated that both nesting status, an indicator of territory-holding power and reproductive state, and the sex of a conspecific audience lead to differences in male behavior during aggressive encounters. Since behavioral changes have already been demonstrated, we chose to investigate whether 11KT levels were also influenced by nesting status and audience presence as 11KT both stimulates, and is stimulated by, reproductive and aggressive behaviors in male teleosts. Male 11KT levels were measured from water samples taken from containers holding fish both before and after interaction. Males interacted under three treatment conditions: no audience, female audience, and male audience. Within these treatments were two nest paradigms: both males had nests or neither male had a nest. 11KT levels varied depending on nesting status and audience type. In general, 11KT levels were lower in interacting males when a female audience was present or when males had nests. Overall, 11KT showed increases or decreases as aggression increased or decreased, as shown by already established behavioral findings [see Dzieweczynski T.L., Green T.M., Earley R.L., Rowland W.J., 2005. Audience effect is context dependent in Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Behav. Ecol. 16, 1025–1030; Doutrelant, C., McGregor, P.K., Oliveira, R.F., 2001. Effect of an audience on intrasexual communication in male Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Behav. Ecol. 12, 283–286.]. Our results suggest that 11KT levels are influenced by reproductive status, as indicated by nest ownership, and audience presence and are most likely modulated by territorial behavior and social environment.

Introduction

While androgens are typically thought of as controlling seasonal changes in male reproductive behavior, studies in recent years have demonstrated that androgen levels can be influenced by interactions with conspecifics. Social interactions have been found to influence reproduction through a number of ways including regulating ovulation (Cheng, 1986, Stern and McClintock, 1998) and producing dramatic transformations such as sex changes (wrasse (Thalassoma dupperrey) Larson et al., 2003; clownfish Buston, 2003). Sexual maturation may be suppressed in the presence of dominant individuals in a number of social species including an African cichlid fish, Haplochromis burtoni (e.g., Davis and Fernald, 1990, Sapolsky, 1993). As a consequence, dominant members of a hierarchy experience greater reproductive success than subordinate animals (e.g., primates, Ellis, 1995; dwarf mongeese, Creel et al., 2002; African cichlid fish (Haplochromis burtoni), Hofmann et al., 1999). In all vertebrates, final regulation of androgen levels occurs in the hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal (HPG) axis, beginning with the GnRH neurons, which are directly influenced by aggressive interactions (e.g., Francis et al., 1993). This indicates that some form of reciprocal relationship likely exists between androgens and behavior and may provide a way for individuals to adjust to changes in their social environment (see Oliveira et al., 2002 for review in teleosts).

According to the challenge hypothesis (Wingfield et al., 1990), interactions between males should stimulate the production of androgens within these males; in turn, the amount of androgen stimulation should be a product of the stability of a male’s social environment. Under this hypothesis, we would expect males of higher social status to have greater androgen levels because dominant/territorial males must be more aggressive in order to maintain their territories. We would also expect these levels to be highest during periods of social instability, such as initial territory formation or the establishment of a dominance hierarchy (e.g. Cardwell and Liley, 1991, Pankhurst and Barnett, 1993). In contrast, when the social environment is relatively stable, levels of aggression will drop and androgen levels will no longer be closely associated with aggression levels. Finally, androgen levels should be negatively correlated with the degree of paternal care; this is supported by the finding that males in the parental phase of a breeding cycle tend to decrease their androgen levels (e.g. Townsend and Moger, 1987). These predictions would suggest that manipulating the reproductive context (i.e., presence or absence of a nest) during an interaction between males may impact the androgen levels of male Siamese fighting fish.

In addition, manipulating the audience type should also affect androgen levels. Audience, or bystander effects, on behavior are well-documented (e.g., Cheney and Seyfarth, 1985, Evans and Marler, 1992, Mennill and Ratcliffe, 2004). They are often sex-specific and can cause both individuals that are being watched and those watching the interaction, known as eavesdroppers, to modify their behavior (e.g., chickens, Evans and Marler, 1994; lions, Grinnell and McComb, 2001; stickleback, Dzieweczynski and Rowland, 2004; Bettas, Doutrelant et al., 2001, Oliveira et al., 1998). Many of the studies on bystander effects have demonstrated that male–male interactions are affected by the presence of an audience in Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens (e.g., Doutrelant et al., 2001, Matos and McGregor, 2002, McGregor et al., 2001). Whether or not a male has a nest also influences the costs and benefits of being aggressive in the presence of an audience. For example, males that have invested energy into producing a nest are less likely to fight in the presence of a male audience, perhaps because the risk of losing a territory to a potential usurper is too great (Dzieweczynski et al., 2005). It would seem logical that hormone levels would change as a result of these behavioral changes. While it has been demonstrated that watching fights leads to elevated hormone levels in a male eavesdropper cichlid fish (Oliveira et al., 2001), we are unaware of any studies in Siamese fighting fish that investigate how hormonal levels amongst interacting males might be affected by being watched.

Siamese fighting fish males are known for their coloration, long fins, and highly conspicuous and stereotypical displays. Males direct aggressive behaviors such as bites, gill flares, and tail beats at other males when fighting (Simpson, 1968) and use many of these same behaviors when they are interacting with females. However, the frequency and intensity of the display elements may differ (Simpson, 1968). Given these findings, one would expect that the type of individuals (i.e., bystanders) that are near a given male would affect his behavior. Males compete for territories and, once they have obtained a territory, build a nest of mucous-coated air bubbles on the water surface. This nest will serve to entice females as well as provide a home for eggs and newly hatched fry (Simpson, 1968). Building a nest and establishing a territory is presumably energetically costly and nesting status influences male behavior in aggressive interactions (Dzieweczynski et al., 2005).

Because we previously demonstrated (Dzieweczynski et al., 2005) behavioral differences when audience and reproductive context were varied in Siamese fighting fish, we were interested in investigating whether hormonal changes accompanied these behavioral changes. We chose to assay 11-ketotestosterone (11KT), a principle fish androgen, because other studies have found that 11KT is an influencer of, and is influenced by, aggressive interactions in fish (e.g., Fostier et al., 1983, Hay and Pankhurst, 2005, Liley and Stacey, 1983, Oliveira et al., 1996). Circulating plasma levels of 11KT are also higher in males while testosterone levels are generally similar between males and females, indicating that 11KT is likely important in modifying male behavior (Borg, 1994). If androgens are produced in response to social interactions, we would expect that hormone levels would change after an interaction. In addition, changing the context of an interaction by altering the sex of an audience and male nesting status should influence male androgen levels after an interaction. We predicted that hormone levels would be higher in males after interacting than before. We also predicted that males with nests would have lower 11KT levels than those without nests, and that males interacting in the presence of an audience would have elevated 11KT levels compared to males interacting without an audience.

Section snippets

Animals

Siamese fighting fish subjects were obtained from a commercial distributor in Indianapolis, Indiana in May 2003. Males were measured for standard length (distance between the mouth and the base of the tail) and weighed (g) before being placed into 500 ml opaque containers to prevent exposure to other males prior to testing. Subjects were maintained at 24.4 °C and a 16L/8D photoperiod throughout the testing period. Subjects were fed Tetra BettaMin once a day.

Experimental design

For a behavioral trial, males were

Results

Whether or not males had nests influenced 11KT levels (ANOVA: F1,64 = 10.887, P = 0.002), with males without nests having higher 11KT levels than males with nests (Holm–Sidak: t = 3.30, P = 0.002). The type of audience present also influenced 11KT levels (ANOVA: F2,64 = 4.314, P = 0.017); 11KT levels were higher in males with a male or no audience present than in males with a female audience present (Holm–Sidak: t  2.187, P  0.032). However, since a strong nest × audience interaction was found (ANOVA: F2,64 = 

Discussion

Our results demonstrate that both the sex of the audience and nesting status influence 11KT levels in interacting males (Table 2). Our hypothesis that males with nests would have lower 11KT levels was supported. The highest 11KT levels were generally found in males that did not have nests. The exception to this was for males without nests that interacted with a female audience. Our hypothesis that 11KT levels would be higher with an audience present was partly supported; type of audience

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Rich Granquist, Rick Alvey, Heather Bleakley, Troy Smith, and Emilia Martins, and two anonymous reviewers for their assistance during the preparation of this manuscript. We also thank Matthew Grober’s laboratory for assistance with the steroid extraction protocol. This study used the facilities of the Animal Behavior lab within the Center of the Integrative Study of Animal Behavior (CISAB) and was supported by grants from CISAB and the Indiana Academy of Sciences.

References (39)

Cited by (49)

  • Selection for winners impacts the endocrine system in the Siamese fighting fish

    2022, General and Comparative Endocrinology
    Citation Excerpt :

    The increase in androgen levels after the mirror-challenge is corroborated by a previous study in B. splendens males where it was described a clear post-fight surge in both T and KT levels after a mirror or live conspecific interaction (Ramos et al., 2021). They contrast, however, with a previous work with males of this species that failed to find a significant variation in water-borne KT levels after an interaction with a conspecific behind a transparent partition (Dzieweczynski et al., 2006). Possible reasons for this discrepancy are discussed in Ramos et al. (2021) and relate to methodological issues of measuring this hormone in water in the Dzieweczynski et al. (2006) study.

  • Androgens and corticosteroids increase in response to mirror images and interacting conspecifics in males of the Siamese fighting fish Betta splendens

    2021, Hormones and Behavior
    Citation Excerpt :

    In nature, males establish territories from where they attract females with courtship displays and where they build and fiercely defend a bubble nest where eggs are deposited after mating (Jaroensutasinee and Jaroensutansinee, 2001). The social modulation of aggressive displays is complex and males have been found to adjust their behavior according to the presence and type of an audience (Doutrelant et al., 2001; Dzieweczynski et al., 2006; Matos and Mcgregor, 2002) and to be able to extract relevant information from watching fights (McGregor et al., 2001; Oliveira et al., 1998). Further, due to its high aggression levels, the species has been selected across centuries in Southeast Asia for cultural purposes where paired-fights between males are staged, originating “fighter” lines, more aggressive than wild-types (Ramos and Gonçalves, 2019; Verbeek et al., 2007).

  • Smaller rival males do not affect male mate choice or cortisol but do affect 11-ketotestosterone in a unisexual-bisexual mating complex of fish

    2019, Behavioural Processes
    Citation Excerpt :

    Males of several cichlid species show increases in 11-KT after one hour of exposure to a simulated territorial intruder (Hirschenhauser et al., 2004), and shoaling male zebrafish have increased 11-KT release rates 30 min after males engage with rival males (Teles and Oliveira, 2016). However, there is no difference in 11-KT of nest-holding male Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens, 20 min after treatment with or without a male audience, but 11-KT is significantly lower in the presence of a female audience (Dzieweczynski et al., 2006). These studies suggest that changes in 11-KT can occur at relatively shorter time scales in response to the presence of social rivals or mates, but our study suggests that overall release rates of 11-KT decrease after longer time periods, which can mask any effects of social rivals on male release rates of androgens.

  • Multimodal stimuli regulate reproductive behavior and physiology in male túngara frogs

    2019, Hormones and Behavior
    Citation Excerpt :

    The timing and expression of reproductive behavior is regulated by contextual information provided by both external sensory stimulation and internal hormonal correlates (Adkins-Regan, 2005). Both cyclical changes in the abiotic environment and social stimulation are involved in physiologically priming individuals for reproductive behaviors such as attending mating aggregations, producing sexual advertisement displays, and engaging in male-male competition (Adkins-Regan, 2005; Allee et al., 2009; Chu and Wilczynski, 2001; Crews, 1987; Dzieweczynski et al., 2006; Lehrman, 1965; McGregor and Peake, 2000). For aquatic and amphibious species, the boundary between internal and external stimulation becomes blurred when examining the influence of chemical communication and hormonal pathways on behavior.

View all citing articles on Scopus

Deceased.

View full text