Skip to main content
Log in

Responses of urban crows to con- and hetero-specific alarm calls in predator and non-predator zoo enclosures

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Urban animals and birds in particular are able to cope with diverse novel threats in a city environment such as avoiding novel, unfamiliar predators. Predator avoidance often includes alarm signals that can be used also by hetero-specifics, which is mainly the case in mixed-species flocks. It can also occur when species do not form flocks but co-occur together. In this study we tested whether urban crows use alarm calls of conspecifics and hetero-specifics (jackdaws, Corvus monedula) differently in a predator and a non-predator context with partly novel and unfamiliar zoo animal species. Birds were tested at the Tiergarten Schönbrunn in the city of Vienna by playing back con- and hetero-specific alarm calls and control stimuli (great tit song and no stimuli) at predator (wolf, polar bear) and non-predator (eland antelope and cranes, peccaries) enclosures. We recorded responses of crows as the percentage of birds flying away after hearing the playback (out of those present before the playback) and as the number of vocalizations given by the present birds. A significantly higher percentage of crows flew away after hearing either con- or hetero-specific alarm calls, but it did not significantly differ between the predator and the non-predator context. Crows treated jackdaw calls just as crow calls, indicating that they make proper use of hetero-specific alarm calls. Responding similarly in both contexts may suggest that the crows were uncertain about the threat a particular zoo animal represents and were generally cautious. In the predator context, however, a high percentage of crows also flew away upon hearing the great tit control song which suggests that they may still evaluate those species which occasionally killed crows as more dangerous and respond to any conspicuous sound.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Azevedo CS, Young RJ (2006) Behavioural responses of captive-born greater rheas Rhea Americana Linnaeus (Rheiformes, Rheidae) submitted to antipredator training? Rev Bras Zool 23:186–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp G (2015) Animal vigilance: monitoring predators and competitors. Academic Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Binazzi R, Zaccaroni M, Nespoli A et al (2010) Anti-predator behaviour of the red-legged partridge Alectoris rufa (Galliformes: Phasianidae) to simulated terrestrial and aerial predators. Ital J Zool 78:106–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnington C, Gaston KJ, Evans KL (2015) Ecological traps and behavioural adjustments of urban songbirds to fine-scale spatial variation in predator activity. Anim Conserv 18:529–538

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bossema I, Röell A, Baeyens G, Zeevalking H, Leever H (1976) Interspecifieke agressie en sociale organisatie bij onze inheemse Corviden. De Levende Nat 79:149–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Cocker M, Mabey R (2005) Birds Britannica. Random House, New York, USA

  • Cornell HN, Marzluff JM, Pecoraro S (2012) Social learning spreads knowledge about dangerous humans among American crows. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 279:499–508

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cully JF, Ligon JD (1976) Comparative mobbing behavior of Scrub and Mexican Jays. Auk 93:116–125

    Google Scholar 

  • Elgar MA (1989) Predator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds: a critical review of the empirical evidence. Biol Rev 64:13–33

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans CS, Evans L (2007) Representational signalling in birds. Biol Lett 3:8–11

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans BS, Ryder TB, Reitsma R, Hurlbert AH, Marra PP (2015) Characterizing avian survival along a rural-to-urban land use gradient. Ecology 96:1631–1640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferenc M, Sedláček O, Fuchs R (2014) How to improve urban greenspace for woodland birds: site and local-scale determinants of bird species richness. Urban Ecosyst 17:625–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari MCO, Gonzalo A, Messier F, Chivers DP (2007) Generalization of learned predator recognition: an experimental test and framework for future studies. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 274:1853–1859

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flower T (2011) Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 278:1548–1555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forsman JT, Mönkkönen M (2001) Responses by breeding birds to heterospecific song and mobbing call playbacks under varying predation risk. Anim Behav 62:1067–1073

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galbraith JA, Beggs JR, Jones DN, Stanley MC (2015) Supplementary feeding restructures urban bird communities. Proc Natl Acad Sci 112:E2648–E2657

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gaston K (2010) Urban ecology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gering JC, Blair RB (1999) Predation on artificial bird nests along an urban gradient: predatory risk or relaxation in urban environments? Ecography 22:532–541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill SA, Bierema AM-K (2013) On the meaning of alarm calls: a review of functional reference in avian alarm calling. Ethology 119:449–461

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2008) Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest. Behav Ecol 19:887–894

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD (2010) Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 25:354–361

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Göth A (2001) Innate predator-recognition in Australian brush-turkey (Alectura lathami, Megapodiidae) hatchlings. Behaviour 138:117–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin AS, Evans CS, Blumstein DT (2001) Learning specificity in acquired predator recognition. Anim Behav 62:577–589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin AS, Savani R, Hausmanis K, Lefebvre L (2005) Mixed-species aggregations in birds: zenaida doves, Zenaida aurita, respond to the alarm calls of carib grackles, Quiscalus lugubris. Anim Behav 70:507–515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (1988) How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: the role of experience. Behaviour 105:187–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houston CS (1977) Changing patterns of Corvidae on the prairies. Blue Jay 35:149–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurd CR (1996) Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:287–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen KK, Larsen ON, Attenborough K (2008) Measurements and predictions of hooded crow (Corvus corone cornix) call propagation over open field habitats. J Acoust Soc Am 123:507–518

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jerzak L (2001) Synurbanization of the magpie in the Palearctic. In: Marzluff JM, Bowman R, Donnelly R (eds) Avian ecology and conservation in an urbanizing world. Springer, New York, pp 403–425

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson FR, McNaughton EJ, Shelley C, Blumstein DT (2003) Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls. Aust J Zool 51:577–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jokimäki J, Huhta E (2000) Artificial nest predation and abundance of birds along an urban gradient. Condor 102:838–847

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jokimäki J, Suhonen J (1993) Effects of urbanization on the breeding bird species richness in Finland: a biogeographical comparison. Ornis Fenn 70:71–77

    Google Scholar 

  • Kauhala K, Talvitie K, Vuorisalo T (2015) Free-ranging house cats in urban and rural areas in the north: useful rodent killers or harmful bird predators? Folia Zool 64:45–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight RL (1984) Responses of nesting ravens to people in areas of different human densities. Condor 86:345–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knight RL, Grout DJ, Temple SA (1987) Nest-defense behavior of the American crow in urban and rural areas. Condor 89:175–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köver L, Gyure P, Balogh P, Huettmann F, Lengyel S, Juhasz L (2015) Recent colonization and nest site selection of the Hooded Crow (Corvus corone cornix L.) in an urban environment. Landsc Urban Plan 133:78–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockie JD (1955) The breeding and feeding of Jackdaws and Rooks with notes on Carrion Crows and other Corvidae. Ibis 97:341–369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez-Flores V, MacGregor-Fors I, Schondube JE (2009) Artificial nest predation along a neotropical urban gradient. Landsc Urban Plan 92:90–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Haff TM, Fallow PM, Radford AN (2015) Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences: interspecific eavesdropping. Biol Rev 90:560–586

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1955) Characteristic of some animal calls. Nature 176:6–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marzluff JM, Walls J, Cornell HN, Withey JC, Craig DP (2010) Lasting recognition of threatening people by wild American crows. Anim Behav 79:699–707

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGowan KJ (2001) Demographic and behavioral comparisons of suburban and rural American Crows. In: Marzluff JM, Bowman R, Donnelly R (eds) Avian ecology and conservation in an urbanizing world. Springer, New York, pp 365–381

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Milius S (2011) When birds go to town: urban settings offer enterprising critters new opportunities—if they can cope with the challenges. Sci News 180:26–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Møller AP (1988) False alarm calls as a means of resource usurpation in the great tit Parus major. Ethology 79:25–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Møller AP, Ibáñez-Álamo JD (2012) Escape behaviour of birds provides evidence of predation being involved in urbanization. Anim Behav 84(2):341–348

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munn CA (1984) The behavioral ecology of mixed-species bird flocks in Amazonian Peru. Dissertation, Princeton University

  • Pellissier V, Cohenb M, Boulayb A, Clergeau P (2012) Birds are also sensitive to landscape composition and configuration within the city centre. Landsc Urban Plan 104:181–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB (2004) Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Biol Sci 271:755–759

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Randler C (2006) Red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris) respond to alarm calls of Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius). Ethology 112:411–416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Randler C (2008) Risk assessment by crow phenotypes in a hybrid zone. J Ethol 26:309–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Röell A, Bossema I (1982) A comparison of nest defence by jackdaws, rooks, magpies and crows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11:1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolando A, Giachello P (1992) Interspecific coexistence in corvids in an Alpine valley of Northwestern Italy. Ital J Zool 59:281–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross SR, Holmes AN, Lonsdorf EV (2009) Interactions between zoo-housed great apes and local wildlife. Am J Primatol 71:458–465

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2015) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org. Accessed 18 Dec 2015

  • Schetini de Azevedo C, Young RJ, Rodrigues M (2012) Failure of captive-born greater rheas (Rhea americana, Rheidae, Aves) to discriminate between predator and nonpredator models. Acta Ethol 15:179–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulze A (2003) Die Vogelstimmen Europas, Nordafrikas und Vorderasiens. Musikverlag Edition AMPLE, Germering, Germany

  • Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55:529–536

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stearns M, White BC, Schneider E, Bean E (1988) Bird predation by captive woolly monkeys (Lagothrix lagotricha). Primates 29:361–367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan K (1984) Information exploitation by downy woodpeckers in mixed-species flocks. Behaviour 91:294–311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tryjanowski P, Morelli F, Skorka P, Golawski A, Indykiewicz P, Møller AP, Mitrus C, Wysocki D, Zduniak P (2015) Who started first? Bird species visiting novel birdfeeders. Sci Rep 5:11858

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington MH, McDonald PG, Griffith SC (2015) Within-group vocal differentiation of individuals in the cooperatively breeding apostlebird. Behav Ecol 26:493–501

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wascher CAF, Hillemann F, Canestrari D, Baglione V (2015) Carrion crows learn to discriminate between calls of reliable and unreliable conspecifics. Anim Cogn 18:1181–1185

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young A, Stillman R, Smith MJ, Korstjens AH (2014) An experimental study of vertebrate scavenging behavior in a Northwest European woodland context. J Forensic Sci 59:1333–1342

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zaccaroni M, Ciuffreda M, Paganin M, Beani L (2007) Does an early aversive experience to humans modify antipredator behaviour in adult Rock partridges. Ethol Ecol Evol 19:193–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000) Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 267:713–718

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are very thankful to all anonymous reviewers for helpful comments to the manuscript. We thank the Tiergarten Schönbrunn for supporting this study, Georgine Szipl for providing the playback stimuli, Florian Uhl for providing demographic information on the study population and Alexandra Průchová for help with data collection. This project was supported by Aktion Czech Republic—Austria (Project No. 70-p9) and University of South Bohemia (Project No. 159/2013/P). CS was supported by the Vienna Science and Technology Fund (WWTF) through project CS11-008. TB was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) Project Y366-B17.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Petr Veselý.

Additional information

This article is part of the Special Issue Animal cognition in a human-dominated world.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bílá, K., Beránková, J., Veselý, P. et al. Responses of urban crows to con- and hetero-specific alarm calls in predator and non-predator zoo enclosures. Anim Cogn 20, 43–51 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1047-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1047-5

Keywords

Navigation